中国高校课件下载中心 》 教学资源 》 大学文库

《认知神经科学》课程教学资源(参考文献)[Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M.(2000)]Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development

文档信息
资源类别:文库
文档格式:PDF
文档页数:17
文件大小:318.47KB
团购合买:点击进入团购
内容简介
《认知神经科学》课程教学资源(参考文献)[Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M.(2000)]Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development
刷新页面文档预览

BIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY ELSEVIER Biological Psychology 54(000)241-257 ww..com/locate/biopsycho Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development B.J.Casey *.*Jay N.Giedd b,Kathleen M.Thomas C No Abstract Despite significant gains in the fields of pediatric neuroimaging and developmental neurobiology,surprisingly little is known about the developing human brain or the neural bases of cognitive development.This paper addresses MRI studies of structural and functional changes in the developing hun nan brain and their relation to changes in cognitive proces s over the nrst f Base on post-mort tem and pe e.the pr cortex appea s to be one of the l frontal cortex duri ood.tasks believed to involve this region are ideal for n nr a e mmt e fMRI studies examining prefrontal cortical activity in children during memory and attention tasks are reported.These studies,while largely limited to the domain of prefrontal function- 3ng support o r continued are consistent with the view that incr easing cognitive capacity during childhood may coincide with a gradual loss rather than formation of new synapses and presumably a strengthening of remaining synaptic connections.It is clear that innovative methods like fMRI together with MRI-based morphometry and nonhumar Keywords:Brain development:Neuroimaging:Prefrontal functioning:Magnetic resonance imaging see fron

Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development B.J. Casey a,*, Jay N. Giedd b , Kathleen M. Thomas a a Department of Psychiatry, The Sackler Institute for De6elopmental Psychobiology, Weill Medical College of Cornell Uni6ersity, 525 East 68th Street, Box 171, New York, NY10021, USA b Child Psychiatry Branch, National Institute for Mental Health, New York, NY10021, USA Abstract Despite significant gains in the fields of pediatric neuroimaging and developmental neurobiology, surprisingly little is known about the developing human brain or the neural bases of cognitive development. This paper addresses MRI studies of structural and functional changes in the developing human brain and their relation to changes in cognitive processes over the first few decades of human life. Based on post-mortem and pediatric neuroimaging studies published to date, the prefrontal cortex appears to be one of the last brain regions to mature. Given the prolonged physiological development and organization of the prefrontal cortex during childhood, tasks believed to involve this region are ideal for investigating the neural bases of cognitive development. A number of normative pediatric fMRI studies examining prefrontal cortical activity in children during memory and attention tasks are reported. These studies, while largely limited to the domain of prefrontal function￾ing and its development, lend support for continued development of attention and memory both behaviorally and physiologically throughout childhood and adolescence. Specifically, the magnitude of activity observed in these studies was greater and more diffuse in children relative to adults. These findings are consistent with the view that increasing cognitive capacity during childhood may coincide with a gradual loss rather than formation of new synapses and presumably a strengthening of remaining synaptic connections. It is clear that innovative methods like fMRI together with MRI-based morphometry and nonhuman primate studies will transform our current understanding of human brain development and its relation to behavioral development. © 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Brain development; Neuroimaging; Prefrontal functioning; Magnetic resonance imaging www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho * Corresponding author. Fax: +1-212-7465755. E-mail address: bjc2002@mail.med.cornell.edu (B.J. Casey). 0301-0511/00/$ - see front matter © 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. PII: S0301-0511(00)00058-2

242 B.J.Casey et al./Biological Psychology 54(2000)241-257 Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).with its lack of ionizing radiation and capacity to provide exquisite anatomical detail,has revolutionized the study of human brain development.Other imaging modalities such as conventional radiog- raphy.computerized tomography,positron cmission tomography (PE)and singl oto the ompute raphy(SPECT While latter r tech 4 diatric c pa tient populations whe topes advancement of science are less clear (Casey and Cohen,1996;Morton,1996; Zametkin et al.,1996).The advent of functional MRI has now further extended the utility of MRI to explore the developing human brain as never before.This paper addresses MRI studies of structural and functional changes in the developing human brain and their relation to changes in cognitive processes over the first few decades of human life 1.Brain development:What do we know? Despite the significant gains in the fields of pediatric neuroirnaging and develop- mental neurobiology.surprisingly little is known about the developing human brain.This is particularly the case during early and late childhood-a time when significant leaps in social and cognitive learning take place.In part,this is due to s across this e range in additi to the on this populati example,the vlev brain co i DC,contains only a dozen o so normal brains from roughly 500 that are from subjects between the ages of 4 and 18 (Haleem,1990).Nonetheless, pioneering work in both developmental neurobiology and pediatric brain imaging has begun to paint a picture of how the developing human brain unfolds through- out childhood. Most of the dynamic activity of brain development occurs in utero but change continue for the fi P I years s.By the brain has reached close to 80 its adul L W g rets nn et a 6).This is als t begins well befor birth in primates (Rakic. al..1988)and results in overproduction of synapses relative state.This process of synaptogenesis appears to occur concurrently across diverse regions of the non-human primate cerebral cortex (Rakic et al..1986).However.in humans postmortem data indicates that synaptogenesis does not occur concurrently with synaptic density peaking earlier in the auditory cortex,at three months,and later in the middle frontal gyr us,at 15 months(Huttenlocher,1997).In both human and non- uma the synaptic density pe wed by plateau ph se tha decreases during ch and 1 to adulthood The plateau anc pruning phases of some cortical regions (e.g.prefrontal cortex)in primates are relatively protracted compared to others (e.g.visual cortex)(Bourgeois et al.,1989. 1994;Huttenlocher,1990,1997).These regions of prolonged development are perhaps most interesting when considering the developing child throughout adoles- cence and into adulthood

242 B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), with its lack of ionizing radiation and capacity to provide exquisite anatomical detail, has revolutionized the study of human brain development. Other imaging modalities such as conventional radiog￾raphy, computerized tomography, positron emission tomography (PET), and single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT) use ionizing radiation. While these latter techniques may be used with pediatric patient populations when clinically warranted, the ethics of exposing children to radioactive isotopes for the advancement of science are less clear (Casey and Cohen, 1996; Morton, 1996; Zametkin et al., 1996). The advent of functional MRI has now further extended the utility of MRI to explore the developing human brain as never before. This paper addresses MRI studies of structural and functional changes in the developing human brain and their relation to changes in cognitive processes over the first few decades of human life. 1. Brain development: What do we know? Despite the significant gains in the fields of pediatric neuroirnaging and develop￾mental neurobiology, surprisingly little is known about the developing human brain. This is particularly the case during early and late childhood — a time when significant leaps in social and cognitive learning take place. In part, this is due to low mortality rates across this age range in addition to the rare occurrence of autopsies on this population. For example, the Yakovlev brain collection in Washington, DC, contains only a dozen or so normal brains from roughly 500 that are from subjects between the ages of 4 and 18 (Haleem, 1990). Nonetheless, pioneering work in both developmental neurobiology and pediatric brain imaging has begun to paint a picture of how the developing human brain unfolds through￾out childhood. Most of the dynamic activity of brain development occurs in utero but changes continue for the first two postnatal years. By this point, the brain has reached close to 80% of its adult weight (Kretschmann et al., 1986). This is also a period of rapid synapse formation that begins well before birth in primates (Rakic, 1972, 1974; Rakic et al., 1988) and results in overproduction of synapses relative to its adult state. This process of synaptogenesis appears to occur concurrently across diverse regions of the non-human primate cerebral cortex (Rakic et al., 1986). However, in humans postmortem data indicates that synaptogenesis does not occur concurrently with synaptic density peaking earlier in the auditory cortex, at three months, and later in the middle frontal gyrus, at 15 months (Huttenlocher, 1997). In both human and non-human primate studies the early synaptic density peaks are followed by a plateau phase that decreases during childhood and into adulthood. The plateau and pruning phases of some cortical regions (e.g. prefrontal cortex) in primates are relatively protracted compared to others (e.g. visual cortex) (Bourgeois et al., 1989, 1994; Huttenlocher, 1990, 1997). These regions of prolonged development are perhaps most interesting when considering the developing child throughout adoles￾cence and into adulthood

B.J.Casey et al.Biological Psychology 54(2000)241-257 243 metaboism suggest that maturation of ocal metabolic course of overprodu ion and s equent pruning o synapses (Chugani et al.,1987)with the prefrontal cortex showing a prolonged maturation relative to visual cortex.PET studies,although informative,are typi- cally performed with clinical populations thus justifying their use with developmen- tal populations.The use of PET with pediatric patient populations to understand normal brain develop ent raises questions about the generalization of the results to truly healthy children.With the ent advances in mrico omes a whole new era in the study of the deve To ate,MRI ping huma brain in anatomica udies have revealed som interesting matura tionl chaneranuctur.The most infomative studies todater those based on carefully quantified volumetric measures and large sample sizes of 50 or more subjects (e.g.Giedd et al.,1996a,b:Reiss et al.,1996).The most consistent findings across these studies include:(1)a lack of any significant change in cerebral ofae (Giedd et a1996a;Giedd.et l 199b:Reissa ase in cortical gray matter after 12 years (Giedd et al. an ing ood ,199 Pfefferbaum et 94;C iness et al 1996 Rajapakse et al 1996;Reiss et a 996).Spec ally,subcortical gray regions (e.g basal ganglia)decrease during childhood,particularly in males(Giedd et al.,1996a Rajapakse et al.,1996;Reiss et al.,1996)while cortical gray matter in the frontal and parietal cortices does not appear to decrease until roughly puberty (Giedd et al.1999).White matter volume a opears to increase throughout childhood and well into adulthood (Caviness et al., 1996:Rajapakse et al..1996).These increases ear to be nal i na ure.Fo there appears to he ar ase in er in dors ntal cor n more ver ntal region tal cortex)(Reiss et al. 1996). l otal ter lobe volume appear relatively stable across the age range of 4 to 18 years.while hippocampal formation volume increases with age for females and amygdala volume increases with age for males (Giedd et al.,1996b).This latter finding may be consistent with the distribution of sex hormone receptors for these structures,with the amygdala having a predominance of androgen receptors (Clark et al.,1988:Sholl and Kim. 19)and the hippocampus having a predominnce of estr ogen eceptors(Morse et al,1986 Taken as a whole,th se neuroimaging rtern studies seem to sugges hat some ae-related changes are regional.One such brain region is the pref cortex.Based on the nonhuman and human primate post-mortern studies and pediatric neuroimaging studies published to date,the prefrontal cortex appears to be one of the last brain regions to mature,particularly the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex Are the last brain regions to mature the first'to go?If so,is the prefrontal cortex region mo ptible to the aging proc ss than othe ortical regions?The videnc efron al fur ion is se changes with age (Daigneault et al Braun 1993) impairment is paralleled by a disproportionate degeneration of the prefrontal cortex

B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 243 PET studies of glucose metabolism suggest that maturation of local metabolic rates closely parallel the time course of overproduction and subsequent pruning of synapses (Chugani et al., 1987) with the prefrontal cortex showing a prolonged maturation relative to visual cortex. PET studies, although informative, are typi￾cally performed with clinical populations thus justifying their use with developmen￾tal populations. The use of PET with pediatric patient populations to understand normal brain development raises questions about the generalization of the results to truly healthy children. With the recent advances in MRI comes a whole new era in the study of the normally developing human brain in vivo. To date, MRI-based anatomical studies have revealed some interesting matura￾tional changes in brain structure. The most informative studies to date are those based on carefully quantified volumetric measures and large sample sizes of 50 or more subjects (e.g. Giedd et al., 1996a,b; Reiss et al., 1996). The most consistent findings across these studies include: (1) a lack of any significant change in cerebral volume after five years of age (Giedd et al., 1996a; Giedd, et al., 1996b; Reiss, et al., 1996); (2) a significant decrease in cortical gray matter after 12 years (Giedd et al., 1999); and (3) an increase in cerebral white matter throughout childhood and young adulthood (Jernigan et al., 1991; Pfefferbaum et al., 1994; Caviness et al., 1996; Rajapakse et al., 1996; Reiss et al., 1996). Specifically, subcortical gray regions (e.g. basal ganglia) decrease during childhood, particularly in males (Giedd et al., 1996a; Rajapakse et al., 1996; Reiss et al., 1996) while cortical gray matter in the frontal and parietal cortices does not appear to decrease until roughly puberty (Giedd et al., 1999). White matter volume appears to increase throughout childhood and well into adulthood (Caviness et al., 1996; Rajapakse et al., 1996). These increases appear to be regional in nature. For example, there appears to be an increase in white matter in dorsal prefrontal cortex, but not in more ventral prefrontal regions (i.e. orbitofrontal cortex) (Reiss et al., 1996). Total temporal lobe volume appears relatively stable across the age range of 4 to 18 years, while hippocampal formation volume increases with age for females and amygdala volume increases with age for males (Giedd et al., 1996b). This latter finding may be consistent with the distribution of sex hormone receptors for these structures, with the amygdala having a predominance of androgen receptors (Clark et al., 1988; Sholl and Kim, 1989) and the hippocampus having a predominance of estrogen receptors (Morse et al., 1986). Taken as a whole, these neuroimaging and post-mortern studies seem to suggest that some age-related changes are regional. One such brain region is the prefrontal cortex. Based on the nonhuman and human primate post-mortern studies and pediatric neuroimaging studies published to date, the prefrontal cortex appears to be one of the last brain regions to mature, particularly the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Are the last brain regions to mature the first ‘to go’? If so, is the prefrontal cortex region more susceptible to the aging process than other cortical regions? There is neuropsychological evidence to suggest that prefrontal function is sensitive to changes with age (Daigneault et al., 1992; Daigneault and Braun, 1993). This impairment is paralleled by a disproportionate degeneration of the prefrontal cortex

244 B.J.Casey et al./Biological Psychology 54(2000)241-257 relative to temporal cortex or sensorimotor cortex(Coffey et al.,1992:Cowell et al. 1994;Raz et al.,1997).It appears that prefrontal white matter rather than gray matter may be most susceptible to normal aging as measured with MRI(Svenner- holm et al.,1994;Peters et al.,1996;Salat et al.,1999).This pattern of degeneration is different from that obse rved in individuals with alzhe disease (Stout et al 1996 Salat al1999).If this is in d the then why might the last are mature be the regions that ar e most plast over prolonged periods of development are more sensitive or susceptible to environ- mental factors (e.g.stressors,toxins)and thus more prone to insult or injury from such environmental influences.Related issues are addressed in Grady's paper in this issue 2.What is the relation of brain development to cognitive development? To date,little is known regarding the neural bases of cognition in normally developing children.In order to address the neural circuits underlying cognitive development,a means of assessing,in vivo,the developmental physiological course of the behavior is needed.With the advent of blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD)imaging (Kwong et al.,1992;Ogawa et al.,1990:Turner et al.,1991).the field of imaging has been opened to address developmentally que of bra and behavior.This me gy cap n the fact that its gen ed 1 globin strongly param agnetic 1 Deoxy therefore be use d as a naturally occurring contrast agent with highly oxygenated brain regions producing a larger magnetic resonance(MR) signal than less oxygenated areas.This method eliminates the need for exogenous contrast agents,including radioactive isotopes. Given the prolonged physiological development and organization of the prefron- tal cortex during childhood tasks believed to involve this region are idea for investigating de ent with this methodolo ognitive pro esses that have ex I g m inhib d attentio (G an-Ra 1987 1989).Memory,inhibition,and attention are often treated as three distinct psycho logical constructs.However,aspects of these cognitive processes may be part of a single construct or common underlying circuitry.For example,memory and inhibition are both involved in maintenance of information in that when relevant information is represented and maintained in memory,competing representations or memories are subsequently suppressed or inhibited.Likewise,selective attention and inhi 10 of a in that en we attend to a relevan ent and c competing,but n ss irrelevant events are s or inhibited in favor of the relevant event.Similarly,selective attention and memory may represent a single construct in that the classic description of working memory (Baddeley,1986)includes a component referred to as a central executive that allocates attentional resources to relevant events.Therefore,memory can be defined in part as the selective allocation of attention to relevant events or representations

244 B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 relative to temporal cortex or sensorimotor cortex (Coffey et al., 1992; Cowell et al., 1994; Raz et al., 1997). It appears that prefrontal white matter rather than gray matter may be most susceptible to normal aging as measured with MRI (Svenner￾holm et al., 1994; Peters et al., 1996; Salat et al., 1999). This pattern of degeneration is different from that observed in individuals with Alzheimer disease (Stout et al., 1996; Salat et al., 1999). If this is indeed the case, then why might the last area to mature be the first to go? One possibility is that brain regions that are most plastic over prolonged periods of development are more sensitive or susceptible to environ￾mental factors (e.g. stressors, toxins) and thus more prone to insult or injury from such environmental influences. Related issues are addressed in Grady’s paper in this issue. 2. What is the relation of brain development to cognitive development? To date, little is known regarding the neural bases of cognition in normally developing children. In order to address the neural circuits underlying cognitive development, a means of assessing, in vivo, the developmental physiological course of the behavior is needed. With the advent of blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) imaging (Kwong et al., 1992; Ogawa et al., 1990; Turner et al., 1991), the field of magnetic resonance imaging has been opened to address developmentally driven questions of brain and behavior. This methodology capitalizes on the fact that hemoglobin becomes strongly paramagnetic in its deoxygenated state. Deoxy￾genated hemoglobin can therefore be used as a naturally occurring contrast agent, with highly oxygenated brain regions producing a larger magnetic resonance (MR) signal than less oxygenated areas. This method eliminates the need for exogenous contrast agents, including radioactive isotopes. Given the prolonged physiological development and organization of the prefron￾tal cortex during childhood, tasks believed to involve this region are ideal for investigating development with this methodology. Cognitive processes that have been attributed to the prefrontal cortex include working memory, response inhibi￾tion and attention allocation (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Diamond, 1988; Fuster, 1989). Memory, inhibition, and attention are often treated as three distinct psycho￾logical constructs. However, aspects of these cognitive processes may be part of a single construct or common underlying circuitry. For example, memory and inhibition are both involved in maintenance of information in that when relevant information is represented and maintained in memory, competing representations or memories are subsequently suppressed or inhibited. Likewise, selective attention and inhibition are part of a similar construct in that when we attend to a relevant event, other salient and competing, but nonetheless irrelevant events are suppressed or inhibited in favor of the relevant event. Similarly, selective attention and memory may represent a single construct in that the classic description of working memory (Baddeley, 1986) includes a component referred to as a central executive that allocates attentional resources to relevant events. Therefore, memory can be defined in part as the selective allocation of attention to relevant events or representations

B.J.Casey et al./Biological Psychology54(2000)241-257 245 This component of working memory does not significantly differ from selective attention Perhaps the common component of overlap in the three previous examples of the psychological constructs of attention,memory,and inhibition,is the presumed presence of interfering or competing information.In the case of attention,the interference may be from simultaneous input or output.In the case of memory, interference n ay be due en inhibit to compe emories/r esentations.If there is no interfe ory proc are see not nec and the structs of atter ion an ry may be i distingui ng componen within the temporal attention pre involving present information and memory involving past information.When there is interference from competing sources then the definitions of attention and memory are less discrete in that they can be defined as the ability to represent relevant events in the presence of salient,competing,and compelling,yet otherwise irrelevant events.How do we ignore and select from competing sources of information (stimulus selection)or fr natives sponse selection) or for that ter 01 respon (respon ution)?This que gnore or n suggests th ur at diferen attentional processing (i.e.during stimulus selection,during response selection,o during response execution)and likewise during memory of different types of information (e.g.stimulus sets,behavioral sets).So,both attention and memory appear to involve inhibitory processes when there is interference from competing sources inhibitory proc esses of this nature are of interest because they ear to be involved in both and social learni ng thre ughout childhood ten h e terms or y control'a and beh. regulation are used itory processes in co and (Posner and Rothbart,1998;Casey,in press).Clinically,inhibitory processes are important because they appear to be disrupted in a number of developmental disorders that have as a core deficit a problem inhibiting inappropriate behaviors and thoughts (e.g.Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder,Obsessive Compulsive Disorder.and Tourette syndrome).Interestingly,the prefrontal cortex and related circuitry have beet implicated in these develo mental disorders refron relate to the ormal devel opment of inhibitor processes?He emphasis may well be plac on t he prefrontal corte in supporting different types of information(e.g.verbal,spatial,moto emotional against interference over time and/or from competing sources (Goldman-Rakic 1987;Cohen and Servan-Schreiber,1992).A number of classic developmental studies have demonstrated that these memory and attention related processes develop throughout childhood and adolescence (Flavell et al.,1966:Pascual-Leone. 1970;Case,1972;Keating and Bobbitt,1978).Further,the converging evidence of ization prefr ex thr child. e(Hu ten et al. nd,198 1991, ugan 1996:Bourgeois et al 1994;Rakic et al.,1994)may suggest an importan parallel between brain development and cognitive development.The most impor-

B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 245 This component of working memory does not significantly differ from selective attention. Perhaps the common component of overlap in the three previous examples of the psychological constructs of attention, memory, and inhibition, is the presumed presence of interfering or competing information. In the case of attention, the interference may be from simultaneous input or output. In the case of memory, interference may be due to competing memories/representations. If there is no interference, then inhibitory processes are seemingly not necessary and the con￾structs of attention and memory may be more easily distinguished. One distinguish￾ing component is within the temporal domain with attention predominantly involving present information and memory involving past information. When there is interference from competing sources then the definitions of attention and memory are less discrete in that they can be defined as the ability to represent relevant events in the presence of salient, competing, and compelling, yet otherwise irrelevant events. How do we ignore and select from competing sources of information (stimulus selection), or from competing response alternatives (response selection), or for that matter, ignore or inhibit a behavior or response altogether (response execution)? This question suggests that inhibition can occur at different stages of attentional processing (i.e. during stimulus selection, during response selection, or during response execution) and likewise during memory of different types of information (e.g. stimulus sets, behavioral sets). So, both attention and memory appear to involve inhibitory processes when there is interference from competing sources. Developmentally, inhibitory processes of this nature are of interest because they appear to be involved in both cognitive and social learning throughout childhood and adolescence. Often, the terms ‘inhibitory control’ and ‘behavioral regulation’ are used to describe inhibitory processes in cognitive and social development (Posner and Rothbart, 1998; Casey, in press). Clinically, inhibitory processes are important because they appear to be disrupted in a number of developmental disorders that have as a core deficit a problem inhibiting inappropriate behaviors and thoughts (e.g. Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder, Obsessive Compulsive Disorder, and Tourette syndrome). Interestingly, the prefrontal cortex and related circuitry have been implicated in these developmental disorders. How does prefrontal circuitry relate to the normal development of inhibitory processes? Here, emphasis may well be placed on the role of the prefrontal cortex in supporting different types of information (e.g. verbal, spatial, motor, emotional) against interference over time and/or from competing sources (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, 1992). A number of classic developmental studies have demonstrated that these memory and attention related processes develop throughout childhood and adolescence (Flavell et al., 1966; Pascual-Leone, 1970; Case, 1972; Keating and Bobbitt, 1978). Further, the converging evidence of prolonged development and organization of prefrontal cortex throughout child￾hood and adolescence (Huttenlocher, 1979; Chugani et al., 1987; Diamond, 1988, 1991, 1996; Bourgeois et al., 1994; Rakic et al., 1994) may suggest an important parallel between brain development and cognitive development. The most impor-

246 B.J.Casey et al./Biological Psychology 54(2000)241-257 tant difference between the development of the prefrontal cortex relative to other cortical regions (e.g.visual cortex)is in the plateau and gradual decrease in synapses into young adulthood (Bourgeois et al.,1994).This decrease in synaptic density is gradual during late childhood and adolescence and coincides with the continued develop ent of c ognitive Accordingly with a gradual los rather an forma 101 new synapses and presumably a strengthening of rem aining synaptic conn These processes may represent the behavioral,and ultimately,the physiologica suppression of competing,irrelevant behaviors.One can imagine that a response to a particular event in the environment will be speeded with repeated exposure and subsequent strengthening of the relation between that event and response.At a physiological level,this may be reflected in the observation that'neurons that fire together wire together'.Simultaneousl but less frequent be haviors are ably w d With urre conne on gin to understand cha of information processing that is observed throughout childhood and adolescence. How does knowing where a function (e.g.attention or memory)resides in the brain better our understanding of behavior?One contribution of knowing where in the brain a particular function 'resides'is that it helps constrain our theories and models of behavior.What we know about the anatomy,physiology and connective circuitry of a particular region can provide insight into the behavior or construct of interes To hat cho onstruct un: ory and inhibition)may in part be deterr tions have overlapping or unique region in activity A number of normative pediatric fMRI studies have examined prefrontal cortical activity in children during memory and attention tasks (Casey et al.,1995,1997).In fact.the first published pediatric fMRI study examined prefrontal cortical activa tion in children performing a working memory task(Casey et al.,1995).One of the central themes of this paper is the importance of examining the functional develop- ent of the prefror tal and related ci itr in context of cogni regard to Thi importan a nun s.F as stated rlier, there i and organization of the prefrontal cortex throughout childhood and adolescence Second,cognitive processes such as memory and attention that have been attributed to the prefrontal cortex appear to develop during this period.Third,the prefrontal cortex and related circuitry have been implicated in a number of developmental disorders that involve disruption of attention and memory in the context of interference (e.g.Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder,Autism). 2.1.Memory and inhibition As mentioned previously,the first published pediatric fMRI study examined prefrontal activation in children performing a working memory task(Casey et al., 1995).The purpose of this study,at the time,was to determine the feasibility of using fMRI with children to examine higher level cognitive processing in children

246 B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 tant difference between the development of the prefrontal cortex relative to other cortical regions (e.g. visual cortex) is in the plateau and gradual decrease in synapses into young adulthood (Bourgeois et al., 1994). This decrease in synaptic density is gradual during late childhood and adolescence and coincides with the continued development of cognitive capacities. Accordingly, increasing cognitive capacity during childhood may coincide with a gradual loss rather than formation of new synapses and presumably a strengthening of remaining synaptic connections. These processes may represent the behavioral, and ultimately, the physiological suppression of competing, irrelevant behaviors. One can imagine that a response to a particular event in the environment will be speeded with repeated exposure and subsequent strengthening of the relation between that event and response. At a physiological level, this may be reflected in the observation that ‘neurons that fire together wire together’. Simultaneously, competing, but less frequent behaviors are presumably weakened. With concurrent myelination of connecting fibers, one can begin to understand factors that may contribute to the change in speed of information processing that is observed throughout childhood and adolescence. How does knowing where a function (e.g. attention or memory) resides in the brain better our understanding of behavior? One contribution of knowing where in the brain a particular function ‘resides’ is that it helps constrain our theories and models of behavior. What we know about the anatomy, physiology and connective circuitry of a particular region can provide insight into the behavior or construct of interest. To what extent psychological constructs are unique or overlapping (e.g. memory and inhibition) may in part be determined by whether these functions have overlapping or unique regions of brain activity. A number of normative pediatric fMRI studies have examined prefrontal cortical activity in children during memory and attention tasks (Casey et al., 1995, 1997). In fact, the first published pediatric fMRI study examined prefrontal cortical activa￾tion in children performing a working memory task (Casey et al., 1995). One of the central themes of this paper is the importance of examining the functional develop￾ment of the prefrontal cortex and related circuitry in the context of cognitive development, particularly with regard to memory and attention. This is important for a number of reasons. First, as stated earlier, there is considerable development and organization of the prefrontal cortex throughout childhood and adolescence. Second, cognitive processes such as memory and attention that have been attributed to the prefrontal cortex appear to develop during this period. Third, the prefrontal cortex and related circuitry have been implicated in a number of developmental disorders that involve disruption of attention and memory in the context of interference (e.g. Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder, Autism). 2.1. Memory and inhibition As mentioned previously, the first published pediatric fMRI study examined prefrontal activation in children performing a working memory task (Casey et al., 1995). The purpose of this study, at the time, was to determine the feasibility of using fMRI with children to examine higher level cognitive processing in children

B.J.Casey et al.Biological Psychology 54(2000)241-257 Six children between the ages of 9 and 11 years were scanned while performing a working memory task used previously in adults with fMRI (Cohen et al.,1994). The study included two task conditions:a memory condition and a comparison/ control condition.The memory condition required children to observe sequentially as the 1uo9u7qN。 ever the current lette e same responded to repeats of letters with a single intervening (interfering)letter.In the comparison condition,subjects monitored similar sequences of letters for any occurrence of a single,pre-specified letter.Both the memory and comparison conditions required su cts to monitor s of letters presented visually on at a time,encode cach letter,evaluate sidentity,and respond to a target by pressing a button.The conditions differed in that the memory task required the subject to keep in mind the two previous letters rather than just a single target letter,and to continuously update this mental record over time.These latter operat cent al to the conce mory (Baddeley 6and consistent with our view of prefrontal involvement in the eprese of information over time against interference from competing sources. Echo planar images were acquired on a 1.5 T GE scanner using 5 inch surface coils while the children performed the memory and comparison conditions.Eight 5 mm coronal slices covering the frontal poles were acquired. Images were registered to a reference image to correct for movement using a modified version of Woods et al.,1992 3-D automated image registration (AIR)algorithm.Movement did not correlate with the experimental manipulation,but rather appeared to increase as a function of time on task and was minimal.The average mo ement across the entire study was less than .5 mm with.34 mm of mo ent in the x-dire ction,and 0.47 mm in the y-direction.Areas of significant activation were identified,by performing pixel-wise t-tests,comparing the memory and comparison conditions. Interior Frontal Gyru Middle Frontal Gyrus M Fig.I Fig.I.Location of activ in the middle and inferior f ntal gyri for an th subjects

B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 247 Six children between the ages of 9 and 11 years were scanned while performing a working memory task used previously in adults with fMRI (Cohen et al., 1994). The study included two task conditions: a memory condition and a comparison/ control condition. The memory condition required children to observe sequentially presented letters and to respond whenever the current letter was the same as the letter occurring two trials back (‘2-back memory task’). In other words, children responded to repeats of letters with a single intervening (interfering) letter. In the comparison condition, subjects monitored similar sequences of letters for any occurrence of a single, pre-specified letter. Both the memory and comparison conditions required subjects to monitor sequences of letters presented visually one at a time, encode each letter, evaluate its identity, and respond to a target by pressing a button. The conditions differed in that the memory task required the subject to keep in mind the two previous letters rather than just a single target letter, and to continuously update this mental record over time. These latter cognitive operations are central to the concept of working memory (Baddeley, 1986) and consistent with our view of prefrontal involvement in the representation of information over time against interference from competing sources. Echo planar images were acquired on a 1.5 T GE scanner using 5 inch surface coils while the children performed the memory and comparison conditions. Eight 5 mm coronal slices covering the frontal poles were acquired. Images were registered to a reference image to correct for movement using a modified version of Woods et al., 1992 3-D automated image registration (AIR) algorithm. Movement did not correlate with the experimental manipulation, but rather appeared to increase as a function of time on task and was minimal. The average movement across the entire study was less than .5 mm with .34 mm of movement in the x-direction, and 0.47 mm in the y-direction. Areas of significant activation were identified, by performing pixel-wise t-tests, comparing the memory and comparison conditions. Fig. 1. Location of activation in the middle and inferior frontal gyri for a representative subject and the corresponding change in MR signal intensity as a function of task manipulation averaged across subjects

248 B.J.Casey et al./Biological Psychology 54(2000)241-257 TTETAT7 0-Back(control) 1-Back TTT77777 2-Back 5 3-Back Fig.2.Illustration of the n-back task with memory load varying from 0to 3. children.Fig.I depicts the location of activation and the change in MR signal intensity as a function of scans across time.The increases in activity nicely map onto the experimental manipulation.Reliable activity was observed also in the anterior cingulate cortex.These results replicate an earlier fMRI study with adults showing dorsolateral prefrontal activity using the same paradigm (Cohen et al., 1994)and a more recent event-related fMRI study of working memory (Cohen et al.,1997)shov ral activation dur m rmation wit interferenc fre interveni The simil tributior activity across frontal gyn for children and adults taken from the Casey et a (1995)and Cohen et al.(1994)studies are striking.Taken together,these two initia studies suggest a similar distribution of prefrontal cortical activity in children and adults during performance of a working memory task.However,the percent change in signal observed for the children was on average two to three times that observed for the adults in the cohen et al study.Based on the behavioral data available.the children had more difficult with the task.Ona children 70-75 in the performed at y et al.study while or above 90% accuracy i Cohen et al.experiment This demonstrates the e importance of collecting behavioral responses in the scanner,but also raises concerns with regard to the interpretation of the findings given the behavioral differences.Are the differences maturational or strategic in nature or both? A study that may help address this question is one presented by Braver et al. 1997,and others (Jonides et al..1997;Rypma et al.,1999),which examined prefrontal cortical activity as a function of increasing memory load in adults.They ed the d emory task as aried the tored uentiaiyot snle leters nd respondedon memory 1 as can be seen in The the same as the letter n trials before it (e.g.if n=2,then A-F-A or G-B-G,but not A-F-G-A or A-A).Subjects practiced until they reached 90%accuracy on the highest memory load of n=3.The results revealed monotonic increases in percent change in MR signal intensity in prefrontal cortex as depicted in Fig.3.We

248 B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 Fig. 2. Illustration of the n-back task with memory load varying from 0 to 3. The results demonstrated reliable activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in children. Fig. 1 depicts the location of activation and the change in MR signal intensity as a function of scans across time. The increases in activity nicely map onto the experimental manipulation. Reliable activity was observed also in the anterior cingulate cortex. These results replicate an earlier fMRI study with adults showing dorsolateral prefrontal activity using the same paradigm (Cohen et al., 1994) and a more recent event-related fMRI study of working memory (Cohen et al., 1997) showing dorsolateral activation during active maintenance of stimulus information with interference from intervening trials. The similar distribution of activity across frontal gyri for children and adults taken from the Casey et al. (1995) and Cohen et al. (1994) studies are striking. Taken together, these two initial studies suggest a similar distribution of prefrontal cortical activity in children and adults during performance of a working memory task. However, the percent change in signal observed for the children was on average two to three times that observed for the adults in the Cohen et al. study. Based on the behavioral data available, the children had more difficulty with the task. On average, children performed at 70–75% accuracy in the Casey et al. study while adults performed at or above 90% accuracy in the Cohen et al. experiment. This demonstrates the importance of collecting behavioral responses in the scanner, but also raises concerns with regard to the interpretation of the findings given the behavioral differences. Are the differences maturational or strategic in nature or both? A study that may help address this question is one presented by Braver et al., 1997, and others (Jonides et al., 1997; Rypma et al., 1999), which examined prefrontal cortical activity as a function of increasing memory load in adults. They performed the same sort of memory task as described previously, but varied the memory load from 0 to 3 as can be seen in Fig. 2. The subject monitored a sequential display of single letters and responded only when the current letter was the same as the letter n trials before it (e.g. if n=2, then A-F-A or G-B-G, but not A-F-G-A or A-A). Subjects practiced until they reached 90% accuracy on the highest memory load of n=3. The results revealed monotonic increases in percent change in MR signal intensity in prefrontal cortex as depicted in Fig. 3. We

B.J.Casey et al./Biological Psychology54(2000)241-257 249 tion of memory load (number of trials back the subject must remember)by age and/or ability. Although the Casey et al.(1995)study demonstrated the use of fMRI in pediatric populations,there was no direct comparison between children and adults since these studies (Cohen et al.,1994:Casey et al.1995)were performed at different sites and with differer t scanning arame ers.A similar rking memory task as de- scribed by r et al.(1997) was use to perform a more direct comparison of brain activation for children and adults.This study was an extension of a multi-site collaboration whose primary goals were(1)to demonstrate the reproducibility of fMRI results across several US sites;and(2)to determine the feasibility of using fMRI with pediatric populations. Each participating group examined brain activity in both adults and children during performance of spatial working memory task designed as an analog of the ng m ask just describ ed.In this task,subjects m array of f ry t four box a dot The every two seconds for 500 ms appea red in a new locatio Participants maintained fixation on a centra crossbar,and received instructions for three different response conditions.In the visual condition,participants made no response.In the motor condition,partici- pants were required to indicate the current spatial location of the dot by pressing the corresponding key on a four-button box.In the memory condition,participants were asked to indicate the location at which the dot had appeared a given number of trials e in a 2haok pa ubjects espo nded by ng the location at which the dot had appeare two trials eviously Therefore.the motor and memory conditions are identical except in the memory demands and extent of interference from preceding trials.Further,the number of intervening stimuli to be remembered could be manipulated,so as to vary memory load.Participants were pre-tested outside of the scanner to assess which memory load was appropriate for each individual.Performance of 75-95%accuracy was required,with the intent of equating task performance across age groups.For some participants this level of per required a 2-back condition.hile others 0 Memory Load otoniceass in percent change in MR sigan prefrontal cortex asfuncti

B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 249 mention this study because it is an elegant example of a task that may be especially well suited for developmental populations. This paradigm allows for the manipula￾tion of memory load (number of trials back the subject must remember) by age and/or ability. Although the Casey et al. (1995) study demonstrated the use of fMRI in pediatric populations, there was no direct comparison between children and adults since these studies (Cohen et al., 1994; Casey et al. 1995) were performed at different sites and with different scanning parameters. A similar working memory task as de￾scribed by Braver et al. (1997) was used to perform a more direct comparison of brain activation for children and adults. This study was an extension of a multi-site collaboration whose primary goals were (1) to demonstrate the reproducibility of fMRI results across several US sites; and (2) to determine the feasibility of using fMRI with pediatric populations. Each participating group examined brain activity in both adults and children during performance of a spatial working memory task designed as an analog of the verbal working memory task just described. In this task, subjects monitored a linear array of four boxes for the location of a dot. The dot appeared in a new location every two seconds for 500 ms. Participants maintained fixation on a central crossbar, and received instructions for three different response conditions. In the visual condition, participants made no response. In the motor condition, partici￾pants were required to indicate the current spatial location of the dot by pressing the corresponding key on a four-button box. In the memory condition, participants were asked to indicate the location at which the dot had appeared a given number of trials previously. For example, in a 2-back paradigm, subjects responded by indicating the location at which the dot had appeared two trials previously. Therefore, the motor and memory conditions are identical except in the memory demands and extent of interference from preceding trials. Further, the number of intervening stimuli to be remembered could be manipulated, so as to vary memory load. Participants were pre-tested outside of the scanner to assess which memory load was appropriate for each individual. Performance of 75–95% accuracy was required, with the intent of equating task performance across age groups. For some participants this level of performance required a 2-back condition, while others Fig. 3. Monotonic increases in percent change in MR signal in prefrontal cortex as a function of memory load

250 B.J.Casey et al./Biological Psychology 54(2000)241-257 Coronal View Sagittal View Axial View Y=38 X=41 7=20 Adults (n=6) Fig.4 sagittal,and axial views of brain activity for children and adults during performance of could achieve this level only in a 1-back condition.A voxelwise multifactorial comtiguity threo tho wereused as crieria analysi for significant activity. As reported in Casey et al.(1998),the results for adults were reproducible and reliable across all participating sites.The results,from six children and six adults at the Pittsburgh site (Thomas et al.,in press)demonstrated reliable activity in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,right superior parietal cortex,and bilaterally in the inferior parietal cortex during the memory condition,relative to the motor condition (refer to Fig.4).The activity in prefrontal cortex was lateralized to the right hemisphere,predominantly,and was more anterior and superior than ob- served in the previously described studies results that tial working cortical age attempt to equate bet een age groups by varying me nory load as function of age, than adults on both the memory and motor tasks.Adults performed at near ceiling (99%)on the motor and memory tasks compared to the children who performed significantly worse (93%and 69% respectively).Although children were performing close to ceiling at the beginning of the scan session,by the end of the scan session their performance had significantly deteriorated.Performance by adults continued to improve as a function of time on task.The few regional differences in brain activation (e.g.insular cortex)observed between groups might reflect these performance differences

250 B.J. Casey et al. / Biological Psychology 54 (2000) 241–257 Fig. 4. Coronal, sagittal, and axial views of brain activity for children and adults during performance of a spatial working memory task. could achieve this level only in a 1-back condition. A voxelwise multifactorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted pooling data across subjects. A contiguity threshold of three contiguous pixels and PB0.005 were used as criteria for significant activity. As reported in Casey et al. (1998), the results for adults were reproducible and reliable across all participating sites. The results, from six children and six adults at the Pittsburgh site (Thomas et al., in press) demonstrated reliable activity in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, right superior parietal cortex, and bilaterally in the inferior parietal cortex during the memory condition, relative to the motor condition (refer to Fig. 4). The activity in prefrontal cortex was lateralized to the right hemisphere, predominantly, and was more anterior and superior than ob￾served in the previously described studies. In part, these results suggest that spatial working memory tasks activate very similar cortical regions for school-age children and adults. However, despite an attempt to equate performance between age groups by varying memory load as a function of age, children performed worse than adults on both the memory and motor tasks. Adults performed at near ceiling (99%) on the motor and memory tasks compared to the children who performed significantly worse (93% and 69%, respectively). Although children were performing close to ceiling at the beginning of the scan session, by the end of the scan session their performance had significantly deteriorated. Performance by adults continued to improve as a function of time on task. The few regional differences in brain activation (e.g. insular cortex) observed between groups might reflect these performance differences

共17页,试读已结束,阅读完整版请下载
刷新页面下载完整文档
VIP每日下载上限内不扣除下载券和下载次数;
按次数下载不扣除下载券;
注册用户24小时内重复下载只扣除一次;
顺序:VIP每日次数-->可用次数-->下载券;
相关文档