中国高校课件下载中心 》 教学资源 》 大学文库

《社会心理学》课程教学资源(文献资料)social self——Low and Decreasing Self-Esteem During Adolescence Predict Adult Depression Two Decades Later

文档信息
资源类别:文库
文档格式:PDF
文档页数:14
文件大小:520.72KB
团购合买:点击进入团购
内容简介
《社会心理学》课程教学资源(文献资料)social self——Low and Decreasing Self-Esteem During Adolescence Predict Adult Depression Two Decades Later
刷新页面文档预览

Low and Decreasing Self-Esteem During Adolescence Predict Adult Depression Two Decades Later Uevnuty r encd Previous studies vealed that low self-esteem is pro ed with dep 四。忘 (ph 2 decades Self-es a cur e in se ed as predictor ng the e likely to exhibit s f dep Kevwords:adolescent development.personality development,self-esteem,depression Self- is relevant for a number of important persona tant life outco es as mortality (Mroczek&Spiro,2007).sub- 2010. 80tnite0tdeBoao20oMh&caen therefore tested whether change in self-estcem is related to n and whether the effects of self-esteem change are 2005:Orth.Robins. nan.2012:Trzes Global and Domain-Specific Self-Esteem mental in self-esteem l as cdictor of life an ial be in a spec fic domain (Epsten rch has dem particularly interested in global self-esteem and ind elf-evaluations in the and that these changes exert predictive power for such impor ant for the adol tra s confronting the developing adolescen initiating or examp referencesand their academic achievement and skills 02008 Esp ally in Germany,many s is bas apprenticeships.Furthermore.pubertal changes occurring dur ng adolescence force boys and girls to adapt to and accept thei tNFP1114-56992.9 this article should be addressed to andrea 14.CH-050 Zurich..E-mail to be newly evaluated,negotiated,and built up (Erikson. zh.ch 1968 2

Low and Decreasing Self-Esteem During Adolescence Predict Adult Depression Two Decades Later Andrea E. Steiger and Mathias Allemand University of Zurich Richard W. Robins University of California, Davis Helmut A. Fend University of Zurich Previous studies revealed that low self-esteem is prospectively associated with depression. However, self-esteem has been shown to change over time. We thus hypothesized that not only level but also change in self-esteem affect depression. Using data from a 23-year longitudinal study (N  1,527), we therefore examined the prospective effects of global and domain-specific self-esteem (physical attrac￾tiveness, academic competence) level and change on depressive symptoms 2 decades later. Self-esteem was assessed annually from age 12 to 16, and depression was assessed at age 16 and 35. Results from latent growth curve analyses demonstrated that both level and change in self-esteem served as predictors for adult depression. Individuals who entered adolescence with low self-esteem, and/or whose self￾esteem declined further during the adolescent years, were more likely to exhibit symptoms of depression 2 decades later as adults; this pattern held both for global and domain-specific self-esteem. These findings highlight the importance of adolescent self-esteem development for mental health outcomes in adulthood. Keywords: adolescent development, personality development, self-esteem, depression Self-esteem is relevant for a number of important personal and social life outcomes. For example, high self-esteem predicts closeness in romantic relationships, better job performance, and academic achievement (Judge & Bono, 2001; Marsh & Craven, 2006; Murray, 2005). In contrast, low self-esteem predicts a number of maladaptive outcomes such as delinquency, poor physical and psychological health, and limited economic pros￾pects (Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins, Moffitt, & Caspi, 2005; Orth, Robins, & Widaman, 2012; Trzesniewski et al., 2006; Zimmerman, Copeland, Shope, & Dielman, 1997). How￾ever, little is known about the predictive effects of develop￾mental change in self-esteem on long-term consequential out￾comes. Addressing change above and beyond self-esteem baseline level as a predictor of life outcomes is crucial because research has demonstrated that the self and personality change over time (e.g., Mroczek & Little, 2006; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006; Trzesniewski, Donnellan, & Robins, 2003) and that these changes exert predictive power for such impor￾tant life outcomes as mortality (Mroczek & Spiro, 2007), sub￾stance abuse (Hampson, Tildesley, Andrews, Luckyx, & Mroc￾zek, 2010), or self-rated health (Turiano et al., 2012). We therefore tested whether change in self-esteem is related to depression and whether the effects of self-esteem change are independent of self-esteem baseline level. Global and Domain-Specific Self-Esteem Self-esteem is best characterized as an individual’s global evaluation of his or her overall worth as a person. Domain￾specific self-esteem refers to an individual’s evaluation of him￾or herself in a specific domain (Epstein, 1973; Harter, 1999; Shavelson, Hubner, & Stanton, 1976). For this study, we were particularly interested in global self-esteem and in adolescents’ self-evaluations in the domains of physical appearance and academic competence. These two domains are highly signifi￾cant for the adolescent years because they reflect two central challenges confronting the developing adolescent—initiating romantic relationships and succeeding in school. For example, it is during adolescence that individuals begin to form career preferences and invest in their academic achievement and skills (Steinberg, 2008). Especially in Germany, many students com￾plete their secondary education at the age of 16 and apply for apprenticeships. Furthermore, pubertal changes occurring dur￾ing adolescence force boys and girls to adapt to and accept their changing physical appearance. These changes are often psycho￾logically consequential because one’s worth in these domains has to be newly evaluated, negotiated, and built up (Erikson, 1968). Andrea E. Steiger and Mathias Allemand, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland; Richard W. Robins, Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis; Helmut A. Fend, Depart￾ment of Education, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland. This publication is based on data from a research project funded by the German Research Society DFG and Swiss National Science Foundation Grant NFP 1114-56992.99. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Andrea E. Steiger, University of Zurich, Department of Psychology, Binzmüh￾lestrasse 14/24, CH-8050 Zurich, Switzerland. E-mail: andrea.steiger@ uzh.ch This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association 2014, Vol. 106, No. 2, 325–338 0022-3514/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0035133 325

326 STEIGER.ALLEMAND.ROBINS.AND FEND Adolescence as a Time of Change and Challenges with stable and global causes such as being gencrally inco Given the reorganization that takes place during adol in orde ion.negative the cks(967)cognitive theory of depre ck.De Raed& .08:Orth.Robins 2009)dem Thus ione deve ental traiectories in trated that low self-esteem a ctss risk factor fo 2005.d uations.specifi shown to b ved physical appearance and soci rg,2008:T 202 ski.&Do Self-Esteem Change and Depression together,it is important to consider adole ge for s the adole cnc (c.g s a predictor re that chan d n dual differences ntrindvidoalchaneiniicd加al may hin th onstructs.If self- n is malleabl durin nd dit iduals do not endure de of g t life acn as s study we the adaptation to age-specific developmental task oms as nge iod and a time of heightened self-exploration that laysthe wo dc es later ate it is Self-Esteem Level and Depression Several theoretical models suggest predictions about the longi ents the adoles ent years must be R s the onset and main ce of de ion (Beck.196 0 e-specific challenees adequately mieht be mor ative in ater ntial styles about th ther self-e nd thus dysfunctional attitudes toward themse es ar self see Rohde esteem are less likely to rch for also y Ziv,M after a fuilure del has been e nd th ential styles imply associating lack of success in a specific task

Adolescence as a Time of Change and Challenges Given the reorganization that takes place during adolescence, adolescents are prompted to show increased introspection in order to find out who they really are (and want to be), how they are perceived by their environment, and what they want to do and achieve in their lives (Steinberg, 2005). It can lead to later mal￾adjustment if this process of scrutinization is not successful and an unstable identity is being built up (Erikson, 1968; Harter, 2006). Thus, ignoring developmental trajectories in self-esteem during adolescence may neglect important aspects of an inherently dy￾namic construct (Greene & Way, 2005; Steinberg, 2005). Indeed, self-esteem has been shown to be especially malleable in adoles￾cence (Demo, 1992; Steinberg, 2008; Trzesniewski et al., 2003). Furthermore, the relatively lower consistency of self-esteem dur￾ing adolescence implies that it is more amenable to intervention during this developmental period (Robins, Trzesniewski, & Don￾nellan, 2012). Taken together, it is important to consider adolescent change for several reasons: First, self-esteem and other personality traits are not entirely stable constructs but systematically change across the life span, and especially during adolescence (e.g., Steinberg, 2008). Second, recent findings on the importance of studying change as a predictor revealed that change in certain personality domains influences important life outcomes. Third, malleability of self-esteem and personality traits allows for intervention programs within these constructs. If self-esteem is malleable during adoles￾cence, practical interventions aimed at improving low self-esteem should be considerably more effective than if we assume stability of this construct over time. Such findings would further highlight the importance of intervening early in the life span, so that indi￾viduals do not endure decades of greater risk for important life outcomes such as mental health problems. In this study, we there￾fore investigated the prospective effects of level and change in self-esteem across adolescence on depressive symptoms assessed two decades later, when study participants were 35 years old. Self-Esteem Level and Depression Several theoretical models suggest predictions about the longi￾tudinal association between personality characteristics such as self-esteem and depression (Klein, Kotov, & Bufferd, 2011; Orth & Robins, 2012). First, one prominent theoretical model assumes that level of self-esteem is predictive for depression. The basic idea of the vulnerability model is that low self-esteem causally influ￾ences the onset and maintenance of depression (Beck, 1967; Met￾alsky, Joiner, Hardin, & Abramson, 1993). This model assumes that individuals displaying dysfunctional attitudes or negative in￾ferential styles about themselves are at greater risk for developing depressive symptoms. For example, when individuals with low self-esteem and thus dysfunctional attitudes toward themselves are confronted with negative feedback at work, they may think that their self-esteem depends on others’ approval and thus feel worth￾less even after supportive criticism (Abramson, Metalsky, & Al￾loy, 1989; Beck, 1987). Another mechanism could be that adolescents with low self￾esteem are less likely to search for positive feedback from others (Cassidy, Ziv, Mehta, & Feeney, 2003). Furthermore, they may also use negative inferential styles after a failure. Negative infer￾ential styles imply associating lack of success in a specific task with stable and global causes such as being generally incompetent instead of assuming that one was not successful at a specific task in a specific situation (Abramson et al., 1989). Hence, according to Beck’s (1967) cognitive theory of depression, negative beliefs about oneself are a key cause in the etiology of depression. Several studies support the vulnerability model (e.g., Franck, De Raedt, & DeHouwer, 2007; Orth, Robins, & Roberts, 2008; Orth, Robins, Trzesniewski, Maes, & Schmitt, 2009). For example, Orth et al. (2009) demonstrated that low self-esteem acts as a risk factor for depression, but not vice versa. Finally, Bolognini, Plancherel, Bettschart, and Halfon (1996) showed that domain-specific self￾evaluations, specifically perceived physical appearance and social skills, at age 12 and 14 were related to subsequent depressive mood, albeit with weaker effects than global self-esteem.1 Self-Esteem Change and Depression One unique feature of the present study is that we were able to test whether change in self-esteem predicts depression. To date, almost no research has been conducted on self-esteem change as a predictor of depression. However, considering psychological changes in the study of development is a key component in life span development theory (e.g., Baltes, Lin￾denberger, & Staudinger, 2006). Because there exist interindi￾vidual differences in intraindividual change, individuals may increase, decrease, fluctuate, or remain stable in their intrain￾dividual development (Alwin, 1994; Baltes, Reese, & Nessel￾roade, 1977; Mroczek & Spiro, 2003). Thus, individuals can differ with respect to the degree and direction of change (Mroc￾zek & Spiro, 2005). We therefore assume that individual dif￾ferences in change reveal substantial information that are rele￾vant for later life outcomes because they reflect more or less successful adaptation to age-specific developmental tasks. Indeed, adolescence has been characterized as a transitional period and a time of heightened self-exploration that lays the foundation for later outcomes (Erikson, 1968). Furthermore, it is often described as a vulnerable age period and thus a time of “increased risk for the onset of a wide range of emotional and behavioral problems, including depression” (Steinberg, 2005, p. 69). Hence, due to the magnitude of change and the high number of challenges adolescents face, the adolescent years must be re￾garded not only as a transitional but also as a sensitive period that may lead to long-term consequences well beyond the adolescent years. In line with theory on adolescent identity formation as a prerequisite for later life adjustments, adolescents who are not able to process age-specific challenges adequately might be more prone to later health issues such as depressive symptoms in adulthood compared with individuals who develop a positive attitude toward themselves. Thus, although a positive prerequisite, we assume that it is not necessary to initially possess high self-esteem baseline level at the 1 Although the vulnerability model assumes that low self-esteem is a cause of depression, it is also possible that it is a consequence of depressive symptoms (see Rohde, Lewinsohn, & Seeley, 1990). This alternative model, typically referred to as the “scar” model, could not be tested in the present study because self-esteem was not assessed in adulthood. However, the scar model has been extensively tested in other studies, and the prospective effect of depression on self-esteem is typically small or non￾existent (Sowislo & Orth, 2013). This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 326 STEIGER, ALLEMAND, ROBINS, AND FEND

ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 32 In this line of reasonin with (formal ope tional thinkin and c ontextual change n-estcem e le be a erts.&Casp indiv s with quite lo self-e em can reduce thei lacing the overly positive a ight be eive them as a chall night give adol es as worth ble persons and,in tur enge estion remains as to the mec isms by which dec n (or in the bulk of the tial and several studies have e for an i c. dy weight sh teem durng ac enc and it is po that th trajectory fo nd a ral ch to a as part of be the in th might leam to nce of stre ng mals in the with initial high showing d nd s al cope and in self- s thoughts in turn.may 12 to cess of integrating positiv gn rch has doc self-estee of the numh luring very few asto cope with (a large or m n t 1997d6 that individ who decre ed in self and the subiective experienc of the chal ed ger ith u and Buswell (1999 hanges i self-esteer Finally nces such s a 24 racnderrole dat ch has appearance may steem on adult depression over a tim span wo d ne that 算0nyi vad of The Present Study ring the en 12 n deve when controlling for level of self-esteem in adolescence adult depressive symptoms two decades later.This study extend Normative Self-Esteem Development in Adolescence n addition to examining the relation hetwe n self-estee depression,the present study also provides further insights into the ither high or low level of self-esteem,largely ignoring the poten tSrpmduringad ial cor chces of change within these onstruct s the adol ears (Ro .2005:Rod nge f-est em during ado depr ssive ns,Trzesniewski,Tracy.Gosling.&Potter.2002:Zimmerman et at age 35. econd.no resea ch has tested the long-term effects o

beginning of adolescence. Rather, we argue that an individual’s process through adolescence might be equally important for later life outcomes. In this line of reasoning, even adolescents with generally high self-esteem may be at risk for depression later in life if they decline to moderate levels in self-esteem, or conversely, even individuals with quite low self-esteem can reduce their risk for negative life outcomes if they increase to moderate levels in self-esteem. We emphasize change because it might be crucial to face difficulties, perceive them as a challenge, and then overcome these difficulties. Success experiences with developmental tasks might give adolescents a genuine trust in themselves as worthy and able persons and, in turn, enable them to approach later challenges in a more positive, constructive, and self-affirmative way. The question remains as to the mechanisms by which decreasing self-esteem predicts depression (or increasing self-esteem prevents depression). One potential pathway may be that decreasing self￾esteem leads to the deteriorating of positive beliefs about oneself, which, in turn, predict depression. For example, a girl may enter adolescence with high self-esteem, but due to pubertal changes in her body weight, she might experience insecurities with regard to her changing body image. Thus, she has to adapt to her feminine body image and accept these natural changes as part of becoming an adult. She might learn to define herself as a person of good qualities, although her body image may not look as typically idealized in the media. Another girl with initial high level of self-esteem might not be able to cope with her natural gain in body weight during adolescence in an adaptive way, and thus experience increases in self-conscious thoughts, which, in turn, may deterio￾rate her initially high self-esteem in childhood. Although previous research has documented that self-esteem is malleable during adolescence, very few studies investigated de￾velopmental change as a predictor of life outcomes. Zimmerman et al. (1997) demonstrated that individuals who decreased in self￾esteem during their adolescent years were more prone to peer pressure, alcohol misuse, and tolerance for deviance during the 4 years of the study. Consistent with the vulnerability model, Kim and Cicchetti (2006) found that initial levels of self-esteem pre￾dicted changes in depression, whereas initial levels of depression did not predict changes in self-esteem. Finally, Bolognini et al. (1996) demonstrated that decreasing self-esteem— global and do￾main specific—from age 12 to 14 was related to higher levels of depression at age 14. However, to date, no research has been conducted on the long-term consequences of adolescent self￾esteem on adult depression over a time span of two decades. In summary, we assume that not only initial level of self-esteem at the beginning of adolescence is relevant for later life outcomes but also how adolescents change during the age period between 12 and 16. Thus, adolescent trajectories of self-esteem development should provide prospective information for adult depression even when controlling for level of self-esteem in adolescence. Normative Self-Esteem Development in Adolescence In addition to examining the relation between self-esteem and depression, the present study also provides further insights into the normative development of self-esteem during adolescence. A large body of research suggests that self-esteem generally decreases across the adolescent years (Robins & Trzesniewski, 2005; Rob￾ins, Trzesniewski, Tracy, Gosling, & Potter, 2002; Zimmerman et al., 1997). This adolescent drop in self-esteem has been discussed as a consequence of maturational changes (puberty), cognitive changes (formal operational thinking), and contextual changes (school transitions) (Trzesniewski, Robins, Roberts, & Caspi, 2003). Adolescents typically begin to see themselves in a more critical and differentiated way, displacing the overly positive and holistic self-views (“I’m a good kid”) they maintained in child￾hood (Harter, 1999). This more differentiated view of the self can lead to a drop in self-esteem because individuals have to integrate undesirable aspects of themselves into their self-concept. This process of integrating positive and negative characteristics should be resolved by the end of adolescence, leading to an increase in self-esteem at the beginning of adulthood (Robins & Trzesniewski, 2005). Although the bulk of the evidence points to a drop in self-esteem during adolescence, there are some inconsistencies in the litera￾ture, and several studies have revealed evidence for an increase (e.g., Demo, 1992; McLeod & Owens, 2004). Moreover, we know little about the developmental trajectory of domain-specific self￾esteem during adolescence, and it is possible that the trajectory for these constructs diverges from that found for global self-esteem. Most important for the present study, the inconsistency in the findings suggests the presence of strong interindividual differences in self-esteem change, with some individuals showing increases, others showing decreases, and some showing no change at all. Thus, we expect to find interindividual differences in both the initial level and the developmental trajectory of self-esteem from age 12 to 16. The process of integrating positive and negative aspects of the self might differ largely between individuals. This difference could be a consequence of the number of challenges an adolescent has to cope with (i.e., a large or small increase in body weight, high or low academic prerequisites at school) and on the magnitude and the subjective experience of these challenges. Previous research revealed gender effects with respect to self￾esteem development. A meta-analytic review by Kling, Hyde, Showers, and Buswell (1999) revealed that males report higher self-esteem, on average, than females, with the largest gender gap observed during adolescence. The gender difference may be ex￾plained by divergent socialization experiences such as a tendency to give boys more autonomy than girls, different gender roles such as self-confidence being more valued in boys, or a stronger cultural emphasis on girls’ physical appearance, together with idealized body images portrayed in the media, which may lead to lower self-esteem in girls (Kling et al., 1999). The Present Study In the present study, we investigated adolescent level and change in self-esteem and examined their predictive effects on adult depressive symptoms two decades later. This study extends previous research on the relationship between self-esteem and depression in several ways. First, almost no research exists on change in self-esteem as a predictor of depression. Previous re￾search has often relied on examining potential consequences of either high or low level of self-esteem, largely ignoring the poten￾tial consequences of change within these constructs. We thus investigated the independent prospective effects of level and change in self-esteem during adolescence on depressive symptoms at age 35. Second, no research has tested the long-term effects of This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 327

328 STEIGER ALLEMAND RORINS AND FEND adolescent self-estcem (level or change)on depr n over de as rel m eaminin the fpe sion over years (cf.So 0&0th.2013 Method ng age 12 th ntal stag Participants cific evaluation as p ceived physical Grob,La ch.2002)3 Participants (N or 13T aspect 2.003) 把 1980:N= 1.952).and 16 year data from a large,nat T5tnhodw 1,0 or this study.wefir investigated level and change in globa 1.527 em during ado cenc .other have reported the oppo origin,urban ve us rura place o r no chang at all from 12to16 hool levels ace to thei d to fin ool)(see ed in the d itial level of self-esteem and in the diffe from the original lescent sample on any of the key nd r physica al.2009) ve effects of both leve nd char in adolesce we Measures Global self-est Self-e nd tho decline in self-esteem ove r the Self-Esteem Scal (RSES dep le (0 199 each item on a di The d t rify the prosp of self-esteem devel score (potential =0-8).Kuder-Ric (1937 nd 7 for th Gordor 2003 The 2000 rded Domain-pficsPhysical appearance and ac pared to others I'm retty attractiv (pe ter to m d phy trying hard. an elational status a low self- The s are to the Pe .C .2012:Th0 et al..2010).As cale (-not true for me. true for me n ad The item to create scores ranging from 0 to 6.The reliability totest for pe re included tim ovariates that are directly linked to the h suggests,anegative Scha 2012 O'De ond,as previo trates 200 from L Craven.206).we included the participants'school grades as an 断 ns

adolescent self-esteem (level or change) on depression over de￾cades. Previous research has relied on short-term longitudinal studies, examining the link between low self-esteem and depres￾sion over years rather than decades (cf. Sowislo & Orth, 2013).2 Instead, we tested the effects of adolescent self-esteem on adult depressive symptoms two decades later, spanning age 12 through age 35, and thus two different developmental stages. Third, most previous research has focused on global self-esteem and neglected domain-specific evaluations such as perceived physical appear￾ance or academic competence. Because these domains are highly consequential during the transitional period of adolescence, we also tested models for these aspects of domain-specific self￾esteem. Fourth, the present study uses data from a large, nationally representative sample for the used variables. For this study, we first investigated level and change in global and domain-specific self-esteem during adolescence. Although most prior studies have revealed a mean-level decrease in self￾esteem during adolescence, other studies have reported the oppo￾site pattern, raising the possibility that we might find an increase, a decrease, or no change at all from age 12 to 16 years. More important, we expected to find substantial individual differences in level and change of self-esteem, suggesting that adolescents differ both in their initial level of self-esteem and in the shape and direction of their development. Second, we expected to find gender differences in self-esteem, with boys exhibiting higher levels of global self-esteem and more positive perceptions of their physical appearance and academic ability. Third, we examined the prospec￾tive effects of both level and change in adolescent self-esteem on depressive symptoms in adulthood. We expected to replicate the vulnerability effect, such that adolescents with low self-esteem, and those showing declines in self-esteem over the course of adolescence, would be more prone to depressive symptoms two decades later. To further clarify the prospective effects of self-esteem devel￾opment on adult depression, we included three time-varying co￾variates: peer popularity, body mass index (BMI), and school grades (Cohen, Kasen, Chen, Hartmark, & Gordon, 2003). The rationale was as follows. According to the sociometer theory (Leary & Baumeister, 2000), global self-esteem can be regarded as a sociometer to monitor how much a person is valued by signifi￾cant others. Thus, when members of desirable groups or social relationships value one’s relational status as low, self-esteem should be low too. Indeed, research supports the assumption that peer popularity is related to higher self-esteem (e.g., Litwack, Aikins, & Cillessen, 2012; Thomaes et al., 2010). As peers play an increasingly important role in adolescence, we included peer-rated popularity as a time-varying covariate (at all measurement occa￾sions in adolescence) in our analyses to test for peer influences on global self-esteem (see Figure 1). For domain-specific self-esteem, we included time-varying covariates that are directly linked to the respective domains. First, as previous research suggests, a negative association between BMI and both perceived physical appearance and depression (Mustillo, Hendrix, & Schafer, 2012; O’Dea, 2006), we controlled for the potential influence of body mass index (BMI) when examining the effects of physical appearance on depressive symptoms. Second, as previous research demonstrates a positive association between academic achievement and perceived academic competence (Guay, Marsh, & Boivin, 2003; Marsh & Craven, 2006), we included the participants’ school grades as an annual covariate when examining the effects of perceived aca￾demic competence. Method Participants Data came from the German LifE-study (Fend, Georg, Berger, Grob, & Lauterbach, 2002).3 Participants (N  2,054) were as￾sessed five times during adolescence at the age of 12 (Time 1 [T1]: 1979), 13 (Time 2 [T2]: 1980; N  2,047), 14 (Time 3 [T3]: 1981; N  2,003), 15 (Time 4 [T4]: 1982; N  1,952), and 16 years (Time 5 [T5]: 1983; N  1,790). A follow-up assessment was conducted in adulthood when participants were 35 years old (Time 6 [T6]: 2002). From the initial study participants, 74% (n  1,527) participated at T6. The adolescent participants are broadly repre￾sentative of the Western German population with regard to socio￾economic status, gender, ethnic origin, urban versus rural place of residence, and education level (in Germany, pupils are divided in separate school levels according to their academic performance in primary school) (see Fend, Berger, & Grob, 2009, for details). Study members who participated in the age 35 assessment did not differ from the original adolescent sample on any of the key variables of this study, including depressive symptoms at age 16 or global and domain-specific self-esteem at ages 12–16 (see Fend et al., 2009). Measures Global self-esteem. Self-esteem in adolescence was measured with eight items from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1979). Participants rated each item on a dichotomous scale (0  disagree, 1  agree). The items were summed to create a total score (potential range  0 – 8). Kuder-Richardson (1937) reliability estimates (KR-20) ranged between .72 and .77 for the five measurement occasions. Domain-specific self-esteem: Physical appearance and aca￾demic competence. Each self-esteem domain was measured with six items. “Compared to others, I’m pretty attractive” (per￾ceived physical appearance) and “Even when trying hard, I can’t achieve what others can do easily” (perceived academic compe￾tence). The items are conceptually comparable to the Perceived Competence Scale for Adolescents (Harter, 1982; Wünsche & Schneekind, 1989). Participants rated each item on a dichotomous scale (0  not true for me, 1  true for me). The items were summed to create scores ranging from 0 to 6. The reliability 2 One exception is the study by Schafer, Wickrama, and Keith (1998), who found adult self-esteem level predicting depression in a small sample of 98 married couples over 13 years. However, this study only included two measurement occasions and did not focus on adolescent development as a predictor of adult depression. 3 Lebensverläufe von der späten Kindheit ins frühe Erwachsenenalter (LifE). Die Bedeutung von Erziehungs erfahrungen und Entwicklungs￾prozessen für die Lebensbewältigung – Follow-Up zur Konstanzer Ju￾gendlängsschnittstudie, Entwicklung im Jugendalter‘ (Authors: Fend, Georg, Berger, Grob, & Lauterbach, 2002). [Pathways from Late Child￾hood to Adulthood. Context and Development in Adolescence as Predic￾tors of Productive Life-Courses (Lebensverläufe ins frühe Erwachsenenal￾ter: LifE)]. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 328 STEIGER, ALLEMAND, ROBINS, AND FEND

ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 329 O6o6O6O▣O 白中白中白中白中中中 Ase 16 odel for self-esteem dabeled $12-$16 for each of the thr (SE).domain-s d physical attractiveness (SPA) estion form BDI-V and the 1 from lepres aaimeebR n ex 5 was r .15 (p <.01).Only correl alpha 7p<01 ared ith the,the BDI-o ontrolled for peer popularity when examining the effect of global elf-r orted depression and expert ratines of depress e.g. times as being lik d,th lation both on the level of sum scores .91)and on the anged 0m278o35 level of symptoms( .70)(Schmitt et al..2003).Thus,the

estimates (KR-20) ranged from .65 to .72 for physical appearance and from .77 to .82 for academic competence. Depression. At age 16 (T5: 1983), 13 items from the original Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) were used to measure depressive symptoms (depressive symptoms were not measured in the age 12–15 assess￾ments). Participants were asked how often they had experienced depressive symptoms during the preceding week. Participants in￾dicated which sentence out of four possible answers reflected their feeling most accurately. This was done for each of the 13 symp￾toms. An example item is, “I’m not feeling sad at all” (1) to “I’m extremely sad and unhappy; I can hardly bear it” (4). The alpha reliability estimate was .89. In adulthood (T6: 2002), seven items from the BDI-V (Schmitt & Maes, 2000) were used to measure depressive symptoms. Com￾pared with the original BDI, the simplified version BDI-V only consists of 20 items (as opposed to the original version consisting of four gradually increasing sentences with regard to severity for each symptom). Comparison of the original scale with the new version revealed that both versions correlated equally high with self-reported depression and expert ratings of depression (Schmitt et al., 2003). Furthermore, they discriminated depressed and non￾depressed individuals equally well, and the two versions had a high correlation both on the level of sum scores (r  .91) and on the level of symptoms (r  .70) (Schmitt et al., 2003). Thus, the different question formats revealed similar results. For T6 of the LifE-study, seven items of this simplified version of the BDI-V were used. In the Appendix, we provide an overview of the seven items that were extracted from the original 20-item scale of the BDI-V and the 13 items from the original BDI. Participants were asked how often they typically experience depressive symptoms. Participants rated each item on a 6-point Likert-type scale (1  never, 6  always). Example items were “I’m sad” or “I’m thinking of hurting myself.” The alpha reliability estimate was .84. The stability of depression between age 16 and 35 was r  .15 (p .01). Only correlating the seven correspond￾ing symptoms of the two scales revealed a stability of depression between age 16 and 35 of r  .17 (p .01). Time-varying covariates. We controlled for three variables when examining the effects of self-esteem on depression. First, we controlled for peer popularity when examining the effect of global self-esteem. Peer popularity was rated through peers of the same school class. Pupils were given a complete list of their peers of their school class, and they were instructed to read the list and then indicate a maximum of five peers who they liked the most. Thus, every adolescent received a sum score of his or her popularity (e.g., if an adolescent was listed three times as being liked, this pupil received the score “3”). On average, pupils received around three votes from their peers (MT1–T5 ranged from 2.78 to 3.51, SDT1–T5 ranged from 2.14 to 2.47), but variability in peer popu￾SE: β = -.41, p <.001 SPA: β = -.19, p <.01 SAC: β = -.31, p < .001 Level Change Depression Age 35 Sex S12 S13 S14 S15 S16 Indicator speciic effect Depression Age 16 C C C C C SE: β = -.36, p <.001 SPA: β = -.21, p <.001 SAC: β = -.15, p < .01 SE: β = -.22, p <.001 SPA: β = -.12, p < .05 SAC: β = -.18, p < .01 SE: β = -.42, p <.001 SPA: β = -.30, p <.001 SAC: β = -.42, p < .001 Figure 1. Second-order latent growth curve model for self-esteem (labeled S12–S16 for each of the three measured models) and the prediction of depressive symptoms at age 16 and 35. The time-varying covariates (labeled C) for global self-esteem are peer popularity and for the domain-specific self-esteem (i.e., perceived physical appearance and academic competence), body mass index and school grades, respectively. Main results are presented for global self-esteem (SE), domain-specific self-esteem of academic competence (SAC), and perceived physical attractiveness (SPA). This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 329

330 STEIGER.ALLEMAND.ROBINS.AND FEND high rangingIr all ond.we controlled for BMI when g the effect of e.BMI.the individual's weight ds to an underestimation of the indica 14,15.ndi60 es in thre First,we estimated "or vel and change for global the eategory (e.g 153 cm for the category "height betw cm151- s")as prox culate the would ind uggest that adole s differ in their lopmental tra their grades we exam of level and change on depressive sympiom ects Mathematics,German,and Plan of Analysis Ve used second atent Results Table 1 pres sents the means.standard deviations.and reliabilit tages of second-order late timates bles Because these nodels analyze change at the latent rather l-esem scale erately stable over timewith (age 58 stin sep imally lower to what 998-2010).We used two parcels (sum of four items for self version of the RSES used in this study:however,as research ha l 201)The We followed the suggestions by Muthen and Muthen (1998- 141.57 [age 14-15]and 66 [age 15-161)and for the domain of LGMs w multiple indi 65agc13-14.69ag ver time On theb Firs effeet for the (2 D Thi ating or variables vides a way to statisticall te indic ical annea 001cF=988 effects and error variance.The traditional comelated-uniqueness RMSEA .029 (90%CI [.021,.037));and academic compe TI:12 years T2:13 xears T3:14 years T4:15 years T5:16 years Variable M (SD)KR-20 M(SD)KR-20 M (SD)KR-20 M (SD)KR-20 M (SD)KR-20 ed825=Tine 1-Time

larity was high, ranging from 0 to 18 for all measurement occa￾sions. Second, we controlled for BMI when examining the effect of perceived physical appearance. BMI, the individual’s weight di￾vided by the square of his or her height, was assessed at ages 13, 14, 15, and 16. Height and weight were measured using self-report categories (e.g., “height between 151–155 centimeters” or “weight between 46 –50 kilograms”). We used the average estimate of each category (e.g., 153 cm for the category “height between 151–155 centimeters”) as proxies in order to calculate the individual BMI. Third, we controlled for the grades that participants had received during the 5-year school period when examining the effect of academic competence. Participants indicated their grades at the ages of 12–16 years (1  lowest grade, 5  highest grade). Grades were measured at each measurement occasion in adolescence using the sum scores of the subjects Mathematics, German, and English (potential range  3–15). Plan of Analysis We used second-order latent growth models (LGMs) to test our hypotheses (Curran & Hussong, 2003). These models were used instead of standard LGMs because unreliability of the measured items can lead to an underestimation of change. One of the advantages of second-order latent growth modeling lies within the latent assessments of repeated measures instead of manifest vari￾ables. Because these models analyze change at the latent rather than at the observed level, this approach allows controlling for measurement error when analyzing structural relationships. We estimated LGMs over the five measurement occasions separately for global and domain-specific self-esteem, using full information maximum likelihood estimation in Mplus 5.2 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 –2010). We used two parcels (sum of four items for self￾esteem, sum of three items for the domain-specific self-esteem) as indicators per constructs over time (see Figure 1). Parcels were built using the item-to-construct balancing method (Little, Cun￾ningham, Shahar, & Widaman, 2002). We followed the suggestions by Muthén and Muthén (1998 – 2010) to specify second-order LGMs with multiple indicators. That is, factor loadings and intercepts of the corresponding indi￾cators were constrained to be equal over time. On the basis of suggestions by Geiser (2011), we also specified an indicator￾specific effect for the second indicator (see Figure 1). This ap￾proach, in contrast to autocorrelating error variables of the same indicators (Cole & Maxwell, 2003; Lance, Noble, & Scullen, 2002), provides a way to statistically separate indicator-specific effects and error variance. The traditional correlated-uniqueness models (Lance et al., 2002), where errors of the same indicators are correlated, do not allow for this separation. Thus, error variance and indicator-specific effects remain confounded, which generally leads to an underestimation of the indicators (Eid, Schneider, & Schwenkmezger, 1999; see also Geiser, 2011, for details). We performed the analyses in three steps. First, we estimated level and change for global self-esteem and for the two self-esteem domains. In addition to average estimates, we were particularly interested in individual differences in level and change. Significant variance in level would indicate that individuals differ in their initial levels of self-esteem, whereas significant variance in change would suggest that adolescents differ in their developmental tra￾jectory. Second, we examined gender effects on level and change in global and domain-specific self-esteem. Third, we investigated the predictive effects of level and change on depressive symptoms at age 16 and age 35 (see Figure 1). We included peer popularity as a time-varying covariate for global self-esteem, BMI as a time-varying covariate for perceived physical appearance, and school grades as a time-varying covariate for perceived academic competence (see Figure 1). Results Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and reliability estimates for the study variables. Table 2 includes the correlations between the three constructs and the test–retest correlations. The global self-esteem scale was moderately stable over time with correlations of .58 (age 12–13), .59 (age 13–14), .63 (age 14 –15), and .58 (age 15–16; all p .001). Test–retest correlations are only minimally lower to what is typically expected for global self￾esteem in an adolescent sample (Trzesniewski et al., 2003). These slightly lower stability correlations may be due to the shorter version of the RSES used in this study; however, as research has shown, reasonable measures of self-esteem are even possible with a single item (Robins, Hendin, & Trzesniewski, 2001). The two self-esteem domains were moderately stable over time, both for the domain of academic competence (.52 [age 12–13], .58 [age 13– 14], .57 [age 14 –15] and .66 [age 15–16]) and for the domain of physical appearance (.52 [age 12–13], .65 [age 13–14], .69 [age 14 –15], and .74 [age 15–16]) (see Table 2). First, we estimated an LGM for each of the three constructs using five measurement points from age 12 to 16. Each model evidenced good fit: global self-esteem, 2 (39)  98.80, p .001, comparative fit index (CFI)  .987, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA)  .032 (90% CI [.024, .040]); physical appearance, 2 (39)  89.52, p .001, CFI  .988, RMSEA  .029 (90% CI [.021, .037]); and academic compe￾Table 1 Descriptive Statistics and Reliability Estimates (KR-20) for Global Self-Esteem and Domain-Specific Self-Esteem T1: 12 years T2: 13 years T3: 14 years T4: 15 years T5: 16 years Variable M (SD) KR-20 M (SD) KR-20 M (SD) KR-20 M (SD) KR-20 M (SD) KR-20 Self-esteema 5.66 (1.97) .73 5.48 (2.12) .72 5.74 (2.10) .77 5.89 (2.09) .77 6.05 (2.02) .77 Physical appearanceb 3.78 (1.68) .65 3.87 (1.75) .69 4.03 (1.76) .72 4.07 (1.76) .72 4.18 (1.73) .72 Academic competenceb 4.22 (1.86) .77 4.33 (1.87) .79 4.63 (1.77) .80 4.69 (1.73) .79 4.82 (1.72) .82 Note. N  1,527. KR-20  Kuder-Richardson 20; T1–T5  Time 1–Time 5. a Scale ranged from 0 to 8. b Scale ranged from 0 to 6. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 330 STEIGER, ALLEMAND, ROBINS, AND FEND

ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 331 Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011 12131415 SPA 144 085.090312 ime 器 器 蒙 都 器 留 热 器 极 二16 0%279,48.p<01.C-992 RMSEA0a0 initial levels of how positively they see themselves.More e l03a) mportant,significant the variances of the level parameters were significant for all ve included gender in the lgm three constructs,indicating that participants differed in their models.Model fit continued tobe good for all three RMSEA0300CI023037: 001..RMSEA026 (90.033D.We Level in self-est Change ghe f-esteem than f Variable M Variance M Variance元c rance (B= 0D).and academic 01).Howe no significant gender escence

tence, 2 (39)  79.48, p .001, CFI  .992, RMSEA  .026 (90% CI [.018, .034]). Table 3 presents means, variance esti￾mates, and the level-change correlations for each of the three constructs. The results demonstrated increases in both global and domain-specific self-esteem across adolescence. Moreover, the variances of the level parameters were significant for all three constructs, indicating that participants differed in their initial levels of how positively they see themselves. More important, significant variances were found for each change parameter, suggesting individual differences in change. That is, participants differed in the shape and direction of their devel￾opment. For our second research aim, we included gender in the LGM models. Model fit continued to be good for all three constructs: global self-esteem, 2 (46)  109.81, p .001, CFI  .987, RMSEA  .030 (90% CI [.023, .037]); physical appearance, 2 (46)  100.65, p .001, CFI  .988, RMSEA  .028 (90% CI [.020, .035]); and academic competence, 2 (46)  91.94, p .001, CFI  .991, RMSEA  .026 (90% CI [.018, .033]). We found significant gender differences in self-esteem level; males reported higher levels of global self-esteem than females (  .21, p .001), as well as more positive perceptions of their physical appearance (  .22, p .001), and academic competence (  .22, p .001). However, no significant gender difference emerged for the change parameters. Thus, adolescent females have lower self-esteem than males but follow the same trajectory across ado￾lescence. Table 2 Correlations Between Global Self-Esteem and Domain-Specific Self-Esteem Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Time 1 1. SE — 2. SPA .597 — 3. SAC .504 .315 — Peer popularity .144 .175 .124 Body mass indexa School grades .085 .090 .312 Time 2 4. SE .578b .422 .353 — 5. SPA .447 .518c .292 .597 — 6. SAC .399 .249 .523d .508 .403 — Peer popularity .123 .105 .078 Body mass index .092 .222 .078 School grades .112 .017 .245 Time 3 7. SE .512 .384 .299 .585b .513 .384 — 8. SPA .419 .474 .177 .421 .654c .339 .605 — 9. SAC .341 .203 .423 .391 .333 .576d .529 .436 — Peer popularity .091 .125 .036 Body mass index .052 .164 .029 School grades .032 .030 .194 Time 4 10. SE .454 .385 .272 .522 .448 .317 .626b .455 .460 — 11. SPA .382 .366 .227 .435 .589 .286 .489 .690c .295 .583 — 12. SAC .271 .188 .373 .306 .224 .448 .377 .347 .570d .511 .346 — Peer popularity .044 .064 .109 Body mass index .083 .097 .025 School grades .047 .032 .233 Time 5 13. SE .348 .267 .201 .465 .383 .255 .530 .418 .364 .582b .471 .377 — 14. SPA .334 .385 .175 .417 .571 .286 .491 .681 .277 .529 .740c .374 .596 — 15. SAC .232 .128 .301 .309 .216 .413 .331 .304 .563 .432 .304 .659d .460 .369 — Peer popularity .066 .052 .027 Body mass index .011 .156 .050 School grades .035 .069 .205 Note. SE  global self-esteem; SPA  self-esteem of physical appearance; SAC  self-esteem of academic competence. a Body mass index was not measured at T1. b The stability coefficient is SE. c The stability coefficient is SPA. d The stability coefficient is SAC. Table 3 Model Estimates From the Latent Growth Models for Global Self-Esteem and Domain-Specific Self-Esteem Variable Level Change M Variance M Variance rL, C Self-esteem 3.59 .642 .301 .031 .401 Physical appearance 2.99 .450 .192 .021 .319 Academic competence 2.92 .517 .377 .036 .416 Note. N  1,527. rL, C  correlation between level and change.  p .01.  p .001. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 331

332 STEIGER.ALLEMAND.ROBINS.AND FEND arate nofMIcvite did fcyer the Our third model examined the effects of level and char ceived academic competence on depressive symptoms at age ularity as a time-va with sche time-va and for comp RMSEA=016(CI 1010.0211).Adolescents'percep nce and 001.02(00 C 1 038p. effects.Finally.we tested all these models without the inclusion identical(see Table 4). n depre ssive symptoms at age 16 and at age 35.controlling for ceptually equal sever n th eve ling ver on d not affected by the inclusion of peer popularity as a time-varying RMSEA 031(90%CI[.028.033,and result We next examined the effects of level and change in percei ge 16 and 35 001)and at age 35 (B= .001).Additionally and equal -2578p<001.cH=98 RMSEA020 gina self-es 027 of thai 00D and at age 35 (=00D).For this analysis.we ted at age 35.thus we reduced the depre scale age 16 from earance predicted rolling for age 16 epressive symptoms (se Table 4).The lysis for the two of self-esteer Variable Predictors Dae 3s Without covariates e level atence chan

Finally, for our main research aim, we estimated three separate second-order LGM models to test the effects of level and change in self-esteem from age 12 to 16 on depressive symptoms at age 16 and 35. Table 4 and Figure 1 present the results of these analyses. As mentioned above, for global self-esteem, we included peer popularity as a time-varying covariate, and for physical appearance and academic competence, we included BMI and school grades, respectively, as time-varying covariates. We also controlled for the effects of gender. Correlations between the domain-specific self￾esteem of physical appearance and academic indicated that these constructs differ between individuals, revealing only small to medium-sized correlations (see Table 4). We first tested the effects of global self-esteem development on depressive symptoms at age 16 and 35, with peer popularity as a time-varying covariate. The model fit well, 2 (159)  278.94, p .001, CFI  .984, RMSEA  .022 (90% CI [.018, .026]). We found medium-sized effects of self-esteem level on depressive symptoms at age 16 and at age 35 (see Figure 1 and Table 4). Furthermore, self-esteem change had small to medium-sized effect on depressive symptoms at age 16 and at age 35, controlling for age 16 depressive symptoms (see Figure 1 and Table 4). Interest￾ingly, the effect of self-esteem level remained similarly strong even 20 years after the measurement of self-esteem in adolescence, and even though we are predicting age 35 depressive symptoms controlling for age 16 depressive symptoms. These results were not affected by the inclusion of peer popularity as a time-varying covariate. We next examined the effects of level and change in perceived physical appearance on depressive symptoms at age 16 and 35, with BMI as a time-varying covariate. Again, model fit was good, 2 (145)  257.88, p .0001, CFI  .983, RMSEA  .023 (90% CI [.018, .027]). Adolescents’ perceptions of their physical appearance predicted depressive symptoms both at age 16 and at age 35; for depressive symptoms at age 35, we controlled for age 16 depressive symp￾toms (see Table 4). Moreover, change in perceived physical ap￾pearance predicted depressive symptoms at age 16 and 35, con￾trolling for age 16 depressive symptoms (see Table 4). The inclusion of BMI as a covariate did not significantly alter these effects. Our third model examined the effects of level and change in perceived academic competence on depressive symptoms at age 16 and 35, with school grades as a time-varying covariate. Model fit was good, 2 (156)  216.10, p .0001, CFI  .992, RMSEA  .016 (90% CI [.010, .021]). Adolescents’ percep￾tions of their academic competence predicted depressive symp￾toms at age 16 and 35, controlling for age 16 depressive symptoms (see Table 4). Moreover, change in perceived phys￾ical appearance predicted depressive symptoms at age 16 and 35, controlling for age 16 depressive symptoms (see Table 4). The inclusion of school grades did not significantly alter these effects. Finally, we tested all these models without the inclusion of the time-varying covariates in order to see whether results differed in any way. However, all effects remained virtually identical (see Table 4). One possible critique is the fact that two different measures of depression were used at age 16 (T5) and age 35 (T6). In order to address this critique, we extracted the conceptually equal seven items measured at T5 and T6 and additionally ran the same analysis as reported in this article using these overlapping items, revealing very similar results. Our model of self-esteem on de￾pression using only these seven corresponding items again showed a satisfactory model fit, 2 (375)  920.91, p .001, CFI  .947, RMSEA  .031 (90% CI [.028, .033]), and results remained virtually identical. Again, we found medium-sized effects of self￾esteem level on depressive symptoms at age 16 (.41, p .001) and at age 35 (.36, p .001). Additionally and equal to our original analysis, self-esteem change had small to medium￾sized effects on depressive symptoms at age 16 (.40, p .001) and at age 35 (.21, p .001). For this analysis, we tested the exact same model as our original model, with the only change of excluding those items that had no corresponding items tested at age 35, thus we reduced the depression scale age 16 from 13 to those seven symptoms that were tested in the follow-up survey (T6) at age 35. Analysis for the two domains of self-esteem Table 4 Level and Change in Global Self-Esteem and Domain-Specific Self-Esteem as Predictors of Depression at Age 16 and 35 Variable Predictors Depression age 16a Depression age 35a With covariates Self-esteem level .42 .36 Peer popularity, body mass index, and school grades Self-esteem change .41 .22 Physical appearance level .30 .21 Physical appearance change .19 .12 Academic competence level .42 .15 Academic competence change .31 .18 Without covariates Self-esteem level .41 .35 Self-esteem change .40 .22 Physical appearance level .29 .21 Physical appearance change .20 .11 Academic competence level .43 .15 Academic competence change .31 .15 Note. N  1,527. a Standardized beta coefficients from the latent growth curve models.  p .05.  p .01.  p .001. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 332 STEIGER, ALLEMAND, ROBINS, AND FEND

ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 333 summary.these findings demons rate the power of positive er for adult depression Furthermore we contributed to the by examining the self-esteen Discussion ve otoms two decades later isi ctive data tim ple in of self-esteem contribut global selt em and in tw self-es the vulnerability effect and ror whom owsef-esteem isa risk inte .For exa nle de ng self- f-si changes during ado ompetence might call for a specific interv d ceived academic Although the presc t study included a large sample ncie hould pron incre ses in self- pils sed on self-reports ol/high school e adolesce e often consic nd 0rh.2013) other ific self-esteem change p meters,indi ng th d econ we found gender diffe with (e.g.. ian)mea ontrol for possible lf-repor This finding with veen is un nd poor healti ly to the mo adu dep d.This finding anters Baume 2005)conc 001 et al stdies have span of and self-esteem durine adol cence exhibited more depressive s 5(6 oms two de des later,in adulth Prio r studi ented a icant link be

also evidenced virtually identical findings as to our original anal￾ysis.4 In summary, these findings demonstrate the power of positive self-esteem development to reduce risk for depressive symptoms both in adolescence and two decades later in adulthood. Discussion The primary aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that both level and change in self-esteem during adolescence predict depressive symptoms two decades later. Using prospective data over a 23-year time span collected from a large sample in Ger￾many, three important findings emerged. First, we found signifi￾cant increases in global self-esteem and in two self-esteem do￾mains (i.e., perceived physical appearance and academic competence) across the adolescent years. The question of how, and whether, self-esteem changes during adolescence has been dis￾cussed controversially. According to maturation theories (cf. Gecas, 2004; Harter, 1999), gains in physical, social, and cognitive competencies should promote increases in self-esteem. Another reason for an increase of self-esteem in this sample could be that— unlike in the United States—German pupils usually experi￾ence the transition to secondary school/high school earlier, at the age of 10 or 11. German pupils therefore might not experience the same academic stress during adolescence as American students do because this transitional period has already been processed. More important, we found significant variances for global and domain￾specific self-esteem change parameters, indicating that adolescent self-esteem trajectories vary in shape and directions between in￾dividuals. Second, we found gender differences in self-esteem levels, with males reporting more positive self-evaluations for all three con￾structs. This finding is consistent with current research on the relation between gender and self-esteem (e.g., Kling et al., 1999). For our third research aim, we tested the predictive effect of self-esteem level and change on adult depressive symptoms. In line with longitudinal research showing that low adolescent self-esteem level predicts a number of adult life outcomes, such as limited economic prospects and poor health (e.g., Trzesniewski et al., 2006), we found small to medium-sized effects of self-esteem level on depressive symptoms two decades later. Our findings support the notion that both adolescent global and domain-specific self￾esteem level have predictive validity for important mental health outcomes in adulthood. This finding counters Baumeister, Camp￾bell, Krueger, and Vohs’ (2005) conclusion that self-esteem is not useful in predicting life outcomes. However, as pointed out by Baumeister et al., most studies have methodological problems such as small sample sizes or cross-sectional designs. We used both a large sample size and a long time span of 23 years to test these effects. Most importantly, compared with earlier studies that focused on self-esteem level, we further found significant effects of self￾esteem change on depressive symptoms at age 35— over and above the effect of self-esteem level. Individuals who decreased in self-esteem during adolescence exhibited more depressive symp￾toms two decades later, in adulthood. To date, change estimates have been underrepresented in studies of long-term life develop￾ment. Prior studies have documented a significant link between low self-esteem level and depression (e.g., Orth et al., 2009). This study clearly advanced previous work by further revealing that it is not just level but also change in self-esteem that has predictive power for adult depression. Furthermore, we contributed to the literature by examining the self-esteem domains of perceived physical appearance and academic competence. These domains represent typical developmental tasks to the adolescent years, as individuals have to adapt to physical and hormonal changes and choosing among career possibilities during that age period. We wanted to differentiate between facets of adolescent challenges in order to detect what exactly drives the association between low or decreasing self-esteem and adult depression. This knowledge helps us to better understand which domains of self-esteem contribute to the vulnerability effect and for whom low self-esteem is a risk factor for depression. Furthermore, such knowledge facilitates to design interventions. For example, decreasing self-esteem in per￾ceived academic competence might call for a specific intervention program at school, targeting that particular domain of self-esteem which is about to develop into a risk factor for later depression. Although the present study included a large sample size and a long time span to study the effects of adolescent development on adult life outcomes, several limitations should be noted. First, the measures were all based on self-reports. However, because self￾esteem is by definition a subjective construct, it is impossible to measure it by objective criteria, and consequently, well-validated self-report scales are often considered the gold standard for assessing self-esteem (Robins, Trzesniewski, & Schriber, 2008; Sowislo & Orth, 2013). Furthermore, other methods of assessing self-esteem, such as implicit measures, have been criticized due to lack of validity (Buhrmester, Blanton, & Swann, 2011). In contrast, for depressive symptoms, it would be useful to include informant￾based (e.g., clinician) measures to control for possible self-report biases (e.g., an unwillingness to acknowledge the symptoms of depression) and to account for the effects of shared method vari￾ance on the associations between self-esteem and depression. Note, however, that shared method variance is unlikely to account for the effects of self-esteem on age 35 depression, because some 4 Additionally to the model of global self-esteem on adult depression using only the seven corresponding depression items, we also conducted new models for the two self-esteem domains. Model fit of the self-esteem of physical appearance with only the seven corresponding depression items at age 16 and 35 was well, too, 2 (355)  809.90, p .001, CFI  .953, RMSEA  .029 (90% CI [.026, .032]), and also our findings remained virtually identical. Again, we found small to medium-sized effects of self-esteem level on depressive symptoms at age 16 (.27, p .001) and at age 35 (.21, p .001). Additionally and equal to our original analysis, self-esteem change revealed significant effects on depressive symptoms at age 16 (.18, p .01) and at age 35 (.13, p .05). In all our models, we controlled for depression at age 16 on depres￾sion at age 35, again revealing a small effect from age 16 to 35 (  .14, p .01). The same was true for the adapted model of self-esteem of academic competence. We again only included the seven corresponding depression items at age 16 and 35. Model fit was well, 2 (375)  847.01, p .001, CFI  .955, RMSEA  .029 (90% CI [.026, .031]), and our results remained virtually identical compared with our original model. We found small to medium-sized effects of self-esteem level on depressive symptoms at age 16 (.43, p .001) and at age 35 (.13, p .01). Additionally and equal to our original analysis, also self-esteem change revealed significant effects on depressive symptoms at age 16 (.30, p .001) and at age 35 (.16, p .01). Again, depression at age 16 has a small effect on depression at age 35 (  .12, p .05). This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. ADOLESCENT SELF-ESTEEM AND ADULT DEPRESSION 333

334 STEIGER.ALLEMAND.ROBINS.AND FEND self-report method variance has already been statistically removed both self-esteem domains indeed play a critical o for life prior level ver,even during ado First.depn Indeed.one should consid ating domains o self-c rt to build un stable amilia on.and y(190 rather than a onal skills and ir the ating that self- ctor of depr vice versa (e.g.,Orth folow-up survey.we were to seven items indic uch as ne n the pred res,as they were not mea red in the present study tudies to include clinical me ent tools in order to p or depressive disoners as a consequence of selt-esteem de eloo that has ally overapping with low it would be valuable to have the same m e of studies misht extend For 16 d with be that an BDI:BDI-V) d it whether a dis es have more complex ass eem and of the first I elo psy a time frame of ove 20 years.We could reveal that not only al ml in the p s still two de later.The and not to elf-esteem is especially malleable 35m References L.Y..Metalsky.G.I..Alloy.L B.(1989).Hope s in their perso t to do In D.L. -1851.Hs Y:V B.Reese.H.w otogrlnmirodicianoreearchmChad.Moecy brins m CA:Brook eister,R. to be accepted in order to find a good attitude toward onesef.Aswe

self-report method variance has already been statistically removed by controlling for prior levels of depressive symptoms. Another constraint concerns the measurement of depressive symp￾toms during adolescence. First, depressive symptoms were only col￾lected at one measurement wave at age 16. This design did not allow us to test for effects of depressive symptoms on self-esteem as suggested by Rohde, Lewinsohn, and Seeley (1990). Therefore, we only tested the vulnerability model, which proposes that low self￾esteem is a cause rather than a consequence of depression (e.g., Beck, 1987). However, the causal relationship between self-esteem and depression has been tested in a number of longitudinal studies, all indicating that self-esteem functions as a predictor of depression, and not vice versa (e.g., Orth & Robins, 2012). Second, the adult measure of depressive symptoms in this study lacks diagnostic clinical value. In the follow-up survey, we were restricted to seven items indicating depressive symptoms. Thus, we can only relate self-esteem to a continuum of symptoms of depression without being able to clinically diagnose major depressive disorder, because this scale does not define a cutoff value to diagnose depression. Hence, it may be necessary in future studies to include clinical measurement tools in order to predict major depressive disorders as a consequence of self-esteem develop￾ment. Furthermore, it would be valuable to have the same measure of depression in adolescence and adulthood. Depression was assessed more statelike at age 16 (original BDI) compared with a more traitlike measure at age 35 (simplified version of BDI; BDI-V). Note, how￾ever, that the BDI-V scale is developed on the basis of the original BDI. Furthermore, the two scales have been compared extensively by Schmitt et al. (2003), clearly showing high convergent validity and a similar pattern of external correlates, confirming that they have a similar nomological network. Ideally, however, the two scales would be assessed and compared over time to see whether they behave identically. Future studies might address this issue. Related to this, the question remains whether depression should be defined and assessed as a statelike or traitlike construct. Clinical psychologists usually define depression as a statelike construct, whereas personality psy￾chologists conceptualize depression having both state and trait com￾ponents. As shown in a study using longitudinal modeling of depres￾sion scores across adolescence and adulthood, most of the variance in depression is due to a trait and not to a state component (Cole & Martin, 2005). One further possible critique is that using a traitlike measure of depression at age 35 might cause an increase of the association between adolescent self-esteem and depression. Note, however, that the correlation spans two decades from adolescence when individuals experience considerable changes in their personality and/or affect, to adulthood. A further issue should be mentioned with respect to domain￾specific self-esteem. We used the two domains self-esteem of appear￾ance and academic competence because they are central to adolescent experiences. How adolescents see themselves in terms of academic competence can be crucial when applying for a job. In Germany, at the age of 16, many students finish school and apply for apprentice￾ships. We believe that these experiences are important challenges and represent some of the major developmental tasks during adolescence. The self-esteem domain of perceived physical appearance was inte￾grated in our analysis because adolescence brings along a number of physical changes such as changes in body weight and height or hormonal changes and body maturation. Changes in body image need to be accepted in order to find a good attitude toward oneself. As we have seen, both self-esteem domains indeed play a critical role for life outcomes. However, even though the chosen self-esteem domains are highly important, future studies could additionally test other domains of self-esteem. Indeed, one should consider integrating domains of self-esteem such as perceived interpersonal skills. It is during adoles￾cence that individuals typically start to build up stable outerfamilial relationships such as peer friendships or first romantic relationships. Therefore, these social changes in the environment might affect how adolescents see themselves with regard to interpersonal skills and, in turn, could be predictive of long-term life outcomes. Having suc￾ceeded in building up and maintaining social relationships during adolescence might be another important root of adaptive adjustment later in life. Future studies might also consider to include classical personality traits such as neuroticism as a third variable, as neuroticism might explain some of the variance in the prediction of depressive symptoms in adulthood. Unfortunately, we were unable to include personality measures, as they were not measured in the present study. However, we controlled for prior levels of depression in all our models, which is a central component of the neuroticism/negative affectivity con￾struct that has been discussed as empirically overlapping with low self-esteem and depression (cf. Watson, Suls, & Haig, 2002). Finally, future studies might extend our research by including moderator variables. For example, it could be that an individual’s school grades differ from his or her perception of academic compe￾tence, and it would be interesting to see whether a discrepancy between the subjective perception and the objective evidence leads to maladaptive outcomes. By including moderators, we might be able to uncover even more complex associations between self-esteem and mental health. Overall, this study provides one of the first long-term findings of effects of adaptive self-esteem development during adolescence on long-term health outcomes in adulthood. To the best of our knowl￾edge, no other study tested for effects of adolescent development within a time frame of over 20 years. We could reveal that not only adolescent self-esteem level but also self-esteem change plays a crucial role in the prevention of depressive symptoms still two de￾cades later. These results provide a basis for interventions in enhanc￾ing self-esteem during adolescence—thus, during an age period when self-esteem is especially malleable. References Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Re￾view, 96, 358 –372. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358 Alwin, D. F. (1994). Aging, personality, and social change: The stability of individual differences over the adult life span. In D. L. Featherman, R. M. Lerner, & M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Life-span development and behaviour (pp. 135–185). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Life span theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1, Theoretical models of human development (pp. 569 – 664). New York, NY: Wiley. Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1977). Life-span developmental psychology: Introduction to research methods. Monterey, CA: Brook & Cole. Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2005). Exploding the self-esteem myth. Scientific American, 292, 84 –91. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0105-84 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 334 STEIGER, ALLEMAND, ROBINS, AND FEND

共14页,试读已结束,阅读完整版请下载
刷新页面下载完整文档
VIP每日下载上限内不扣除下载券和下载次数;
按次数下载不扣除下载券;
注册用户24小时内重复下载只扣除一次;
顺序:VIP每日次数-->可用次数-->下载券;
相关文档